Submit manuscript...
Journal of
eISSN: 2373-6445

Psychology & Clinical Psychiatry

Correspondence:

Received: January 01, 1970 | Published: ,

Citation: DOI:

Download PDF

Abstract

Orthorexia nervosa (ON) refers to an obsession with the quantity and quality of food ingested. While altering ones diet is a common treatment to improve health, individuals diagnosed with ON obsess over his or her diet. Where’s more, individuals plagued by orthorexia nervosa feel badly about themselves if they fail to adhere to their ideal nutritional guidelines. Although little research has been performed to properly diagnose people with ON, this manuscript aims to shed light on to the developing epidemic and will be the first publication, to our knowledge, to compile a list of psychological signatures that may help future clinicians diagnose this emerging eating disorder.

Keywords: orthorexia nervosa, diets, psychological disorder

Abbreviations

ON, orthorexia nervosa; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; OCPD, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder

Introduction

Orthorexia nervosa (ON) is a complex and serious motivated behavioral condition with high morbidity and mortality.1 Characterized by self-starvation, excessive exercise, morbid eating restraint, and overvalued fear of unhealthy eating, ON has a driven nature and bears phenomenological similarity to the addictions.2 Dysregulation in the neurological circuitry underlying anxiety and reward, as well as environmental induced disturbances within homeostatic epicenters, are implicated in the etiology and maintenance of ON.3-7 Most of these physiological changes are state-related and reversible; however growing evidence suggests some feed forward, contributing to entrainment and maintenance of excessive food restriction and exercise behaviors. Interesting most cases of ON occur in females.8–10 Although little is known, we speculate that the relatively narrow peripubertal window of onset.7 and marked female preponderance suggest a role for estrogen in facilitating development of the disorder. The mechanism for this relationship may pertain to estrogen's regulation of gene transcription within neurotransmitter systems relevant to food reward, stress vulnerability or anxiety and is likely to differ in new onset and chronic cases. Trait anxiety has been linked to risk for eating disorders.11,12 to predisposition to anxiety disorders.13 and to impaired fear conditioning, fear extinction or safety learning.14-17 Impaired fear conditioning has in turn, been proposed as a potential mechanism to explain the rapid development and persistence of calorie restriction and fear of unhealthy foods in ON.18

Despite ON's high morbidity and mortality, effective treatments are limited, with the best evidence supporting family-based therapy for adolescent ON.19,20 Better prognosis in this age group is likely due in part to a higher percentage of milder self-limited cases. In chronic adult ON, by comparison, no randomized controlled treatment trial has demonstrated efficacy in achieving remission.21,22 Furthermore, six month relapse rates following inpatient weight restoration are close to 50%.23 and.24 Once established, restrictive eating patterns are hard to extinguish and tend to persist.25 and limited dietary variety at discharge in weight-restored patients has been found to predict relapse 1-year post-hospitalization.26 Surprisingly, however, little research has focused on interventions targeted at relapse prevention in ON following weight restoration.

This review will focus on the relationship between ON and psychological cues for clinical diagnosis (Figure 1). In addition, this review will take a translational approach into addressing the pathophysiology underlying ON and its maintenance, and the potential development of improved treatments for this condition.

Figure 1 Elementary model of orthorexia nervosa psychological manifestation.

Results and discussion

Successful treatment in addiction involves prevention of a rewarding outcome and abstinence from the drug of abuse, whereas in anxiety disorders treatment requires removal of an aversive outcome and fear extinction. The successful treatment of ON may well require both interruption of excessive exercise and dietary restraint, as well as extinction of the heightened anxiety and meal avoidant behaviors that characterize the disorder. People suffering from ON commonly display the following signs:

  1. Prolonged contemplation of healthy food choices (>3 hr/day)
  2. Planning advanced menus
  3. Continually limiting the number of allowed foods
  4. Experiencing a reduced quality of life, social isolation, and depression
  5. Critical of other diets
  6. Overcome with guilt or self-loathing when they fail at dieting

In this review, we have synthesized existing research to identify what is known about the symptoms, prevalence, neuropsychological profile, and treatment of ON. An examination of diagnostic boundaries reveals important points of symptom overlap between ON, obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD), somatic symptom disorder, illness anxiety disorder, and psychotic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychological data suggest that ON symptoms are independently associated with key facets of executive dysfunction for which some of these conditions already overlap. Discussion of cognitive weaknesses in set-shifting, external attention, and working memory highlights the value of continued research to identify intermediate, transdiagnostic endophenotypes for insight into the neuropathogenesis of orthorexia. An evaluation of current orthorexia measures indicates a need for further psychometric development to ensure that subsequent research has access to reliable and valid assessment tools. Optimized assessment will not only permit a clearer understanding of prevalence rates, psychosocial risk factors, and comorbid psychopathology but will also be needed to index intervention effectiveness. Though the field lacks data on therapeutic outcomes, current best practices suggest that orthorexia can successfully be treated with a combination of cognitive-behavioral therapy, psychoeducation, and medication.

Conclusion

Recent research in the fields of the neuroscience of learning and psychology of addiction has informed our understanding of ON as a motivated behavioral disorder. Both anxiety disorders and an anxious temperament have long been recognized as highly comorbid with ON; however, only recently has research sought to address the mechanisms underlying this relationship. Work in the neuroscience of learning, fear conditioning, and animal models of ON is contributing to a new understanding of the complex etiology and maintenance of this disorder. A major challenge in severe and chronic ON is the development of effective relapse prevention interventions following hospital-based diet restoration. Treatments targeting anxiety and fear extinction in recently diet restored patients may therefore be of promise. The successful generalization of normal eating behavior and food choice across social settings is likely to be especially important in helping chronically ill patients recover from this serious and debilitating disorder.

Acknowledgments

This work was facilitated by the Faulkner University, Department of Psychology, and Faulkner University College of Medicine. Funding for this work was provided in part by Max Credit Unions Improving Science Fund (392485) and partially supplemented by Faulkner Universities “Go Eagle Research Go” faculty endowment.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Neppe VM. Classifying déjà vu linked with déjà experiences(Section 4). IQNexus Journal. 2015;7(1):25-31.
  2. Neppe VM. A study of déjà vu experience: thesis. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University of the Witwatersrand, Johannesburg, Africa, 1981.
  3. Neppe VM. The Psychology of Déjà Vu: Have I been Here Before? Johannesburg Witwatersrand University Press, 1983.
  4. Neppe VM. Is déjà vu a symptom of temporal lobe epilepsy?S Afr Med J. 1981;60(23):907-908.
  5. Neppe VM. Déjà vu in the survival context. Theta. 1986;13/14(2):26-29.
  6. Neppe VM. Funkhouser ATs Déjà Vu: A Second Look. Brainvoyage.com, Seattle, USA; 2006.
  7. Neppe VM. The concept of déjà vu. Parapsychological Journal of South Africa. 1983;4(1):1-10.
  8. Neppe VM. Does the definition of déjà vu withstand a quarterof a century of research? Déjà Vu: A Second Look (Chapter 15), Brainvoyage.com. Seattle, USA, 2006.
  9. Neppe VM. The qualitative aspects of déjà vu experience. Déjà Vu Revisited. Brainvoyage.com (Brainquest Press), Seattle, USA; 2006. p. 13-64.
  10. MacCurdy JT. The psychology of déjà vu, in Sectionaltransactions: British Association for the Advancement of theScience: Report of the 92nd meeting. British Association Office, London; 1924. 442 p.
  11. Jackson JH. On a particular variety of epilepsy “intellectualaura”, one case with symptoms oforganic brain disease. Brain. 1889;11:179-207.
  12. Wigan A. The duality of the mind. Longaan, Brown, Greenand Longman, London; 1844.
  13. Maudsley H. The double brain. Mind 1889;14(54):161-187.
  14. Grasset J. La sensation déjà vu. Journal de Psychologie Normaleet Pathologique. 1904;1:17.
  15. Ellis HD. The world of dreams. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1911.
  16. Neppe VM. A special note to the reader. In: Neppe VM, editor. , Déjà Vu Revisited, Brainvoyage.com. Seattle, WA, USA, 2006. Xv p.
  17. Buccola G. Le illusionidellamemoria. Rivista di FilosofiaScientif. 1883;2:708.
  18. Sully J. Illusions. Humboldt, New York, USA; 1881. 425 p.
  19. Titchener EB. A Beginner’s psychology. New York: McMillan, USA; 1918. 398 p.
  20. Neppe VM. Déjà Vu Revisited. Brainvoyage.com (BrainquestPress), Seattle, USA; 2006.
  21. Heymans G. EineEnquêteüberdepersonalisation und faussereconnaissance [An inquiry concerning depersonalization andfalse recognition]. ZeitschriftfürPsychologie und der Physiologieder Sinnesorgane. 1904;36:321.
  22. Penfield W. The role of temporal cortex in certain psychicalphenomena. Journal mental science. 1955;101:451-465.
  23. Bernhard-Leroy Étudesurl’illusion de faussereconnaissance. Thesis, Faculté de Médecine de Paris; 1898.
  24. Dugas L. Observations Sur La FausseMémoire. RévuePhilosophique. 1894;37:34-45.
  25. Freud S. ZurTechnik der Psychoanalyse undzurMetapsychologie. In: Leipzig, editors. Internationaler PsychoanalytischerVerlag. 1924.
  26. Ferenczi S. A case of déjà vu in Final contributions to the problems of methods of psychoanalysis. Hogarth Press, London; 1912.
Creative Commons Attribution License

© . This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and build upon your work non-commercially.