Submit manuscript...
Journal of
eISSN: 2574-8114

Textile Engineering & Fashion Technology

Research Article Volume 10 Issue 3

How similar-to-me is too similar-to-me? Do young women really want to be like the influencer in that luxury fashion advertisement?

Zoe McDonald-Mair,2 Lisa S McNeill1

1Dean Postgraduate Research, Otago Business School, University of Otago, New Zealand
2University of Otago, New Zealand

Correspondence: Lisa S McNeill, Dean Postgraduate Research, Otago Business School, University of Otago, Fourth Floor, 60 Clyde St, Dunedin, Ōtepoti, PO Box 56, Dunedin 9054, New Zealand, Tel +64 3 479 5758

Received: May 16, 2024 | Published: May 27, 2024

Citation: McNeill LS. How similar-to-me is too similar-to-me? Do young women really want to be like the influencer in that luxury fashion advertisement? J Textile Eng Fashion Technol. 2024;10(3):127-135. DOI: 10.15406/jteft.2024.10.00378

Download PDF

Abstract

This study asks whether social media influencers or celebrities are more effective in endorsing luxury fashion goods. Using a New Zealand sample of women aged 18-25, this study employed a between-subjects experimental design with participants responding to either a celebrity or influencer advertising condition, using a luxury handbag as a stimulus. Using match-up hypotheses and self-congruity theory, the current study finds that celebrities encourage greater feelings of wishful identification among the sample group of young women, that participants express an actual self-concept that is more similar to celebrity images and that celebrities promote stronger positive attitudes towards luxury products and advertisements amongst these young women. The study provides a novel contribution to endorsement and promotion literature, as it contradicts prior studies of low involvement goods (such as cosmetics or diet products) which find that young female consumers prefer endorsers who are socially closer to themselves. The study is one of the few to directly compare responses to celebrity and social media endorsement of luxury products, thus extending our knowledge of the likely return on investment in social media endorsement by social influencers.

Keywords: celebrity, social media, influencers, endorsement, luxury

Introduction

Luxury brands by design create a strong sense of identity and are heavily reliant on imagery that conveys a symbolic sense of belonging to their brand.1,2 People often purchase luxury products as part of conspicuous consumption behaviour, in an effort to elevate their prestige among their peers and society generally.3 As exclusivity is a key component of luxury attributions, the marketing of luxury goods necessarily conveys the same sense of elegance and distance.4,5 When marketing luxury goods, brands recognise that customer motivations are different from those purchasing non-luxury products, thus promotion is often focussed on desiring a product rather than needing it. Hence, luxury promotion is producer-led rather than consumer-focused and seeks to create a sense of admiration rather than community in order to maintain a refined and exclusionary image.6,7

Increasingly, luxury brands are turning to social media influencers rather than traditional celebrities, as they are deemed to have a greater return on investment and offer a new way to penetrate the market and create gross brand awareness. However, whilst recent literature celebrates the strengths of social media influencers in motivating sales of products, within the specialised field of marketing luxury goods some question whether these influencers may dilute the luxury brand image by contradicting its core value of exclusivity.8 While there are studies that consider the role of social media in luxury brand promotion,9–12 few of these explicitly consider the social media influencer and their effectiveness in promoting luxury brands. This study thus considers the role of the endorser in modern luxury product advertising, asking whether consumers are more likely to engage positively with celebrities or social media influencers when evaluating luxury product advertisements? And, what effect does the use of celebrity or social media influencers have on purchase intention toward luxury products?

Luxury goods and social motivation

The concept of luxury has an inherent social component as luxury goods were formerly reserved for the upper class, and they are therefore associated with social distance.13,14 The showcasing of social distinction is a prime example of the motive for luxury brand purchases.15–17 Social status is significantly related to the perceived purchase intention of luxury brands.18–21 Luxury brands stimulate vertical comparisons, create social distance and facilitate downward comparison.22

Identification with a luxury brand’s image can express a consumer’s need to show their social standing, and can create further attraction to the brand.23 Jiang et al.4 found that the more luxurious the advertisement was, the more rejected the participant felt. This alienating strategy works because as people feel excluded and distant from the images, their desire to amass luxury goods increases, in an effort to enhance their social desirability.24,25 Consumers look for luxury products that elevate them by differentiating them from others, rather than products used by those to whom they relate.26 Consumers are looking to level up and need to see endorsers who represent a level above them, rather than someone with whom they feel familiarity.

The role of endorsers

To demonstrate the value of their goods, luxury brands often use endorsement in their advertising to illustrate the social benefits of owning these conspicuous goods. Endorsement aligns the personification of a brand with the values consumers wish to emulate. Endorsers are evaluated by consumers in how they fit with the brand, how much expertise they have in relation to the product and the level to which the consumer wishes to be like the endorser. This, in turn, influences the effectiveness of the advertisement. Endorsement is considered to be a way of externalising the brand image to aid in the formation of favourable brand opinions.27 Endorsement by a celebrity has been found to be one of the most effective marketing techniques.28–30

Endorser selection

While recent literature has theorised whether social media influencers or celebrities are more effective endorsers,31,32 to date there has been only one study which directly compares the two endorsers.33 Further, there has been no direct comparative investigation of celebrities and social media influencers within the realm of luxury goods. Traditionally, celebrities have been employed for the endorsement of luxury brands; however, with the rise of social media, luxury brands are increasingly reliant on influencers to promote their brand through different avenues.

There is evidence which suggests celebrity endorsements do not always pay off.34–36 Celebrity endorsements, while attracting attention to the brand, also have the potential to overshadow the brand itself.37,38 Literature surrounding social media influencers has shown that they can be more reliable, have lower costs and be effective endorsers.32,33 Belch and Belch39 found that consumers are influenced more by people to whom they relate; hence, if someone relates more to an influencer, that advertisement may be more effective. However, relatability is not the design of luxury brands. It has also been found that the gap between advertisements and sales is hard to measure,40,41 and, as a result, to claim that a celebrity endorser has not worked well may be an overstatement. The ways in which celebrities can benefit brands cannot always be measured, as illustrated by Nelson and Deborah,42 who theorised that as celebrities are admired, this admiration can translate into profits for brands. Older research has found that celebrity endorsement has been found to produce more positive attitudes to advertising and purchase intention in comparison with a non-celebrity endorser,43,44 however these studies occurred prior to the rise of the pseudo celebrity of the social media influencer. Research which investigates the effectiveness of endorsements often compares celebrities to normal people. While influencers are ‘normal’ people, they represent a middle ground between celebrities and non-celebrities. Where influencers can motivate purchase decisions and enjoy higher levels of trust online, this has generally been explored in the context of low involvement goods.33,45

Luxury goods and endorsement effectiveness measures

Wishful identification: Consumers are more likely to be influenced by endorsers with whom they identify.46 Identification with the endorser and perceived endorser credibility influence advertising effectiveness.33,46,47 Identification is a process in which a person takes on the identity of a character, losing their own self in the process.48 In the context of celebrity endorsements, identification usually exists in the form of wishful identification, which is derived from social cognition theory, whereby people change their behaviour to mirror another’s.49,50 Wishful identification is the process of wanting to be more like a media figure.51 Normally associated with fictional figures in the media, wishful identification can be used to evaluate luxury brands as the wishful aspect works in conjunction with the aspirational nature of luxury.52–54 Schouten et al.33 used wishful identification to explain attitudes about both influencers and celebrities. The popularity of a celebrity conveys the image of a higher status, to which people aspire.55 Identification with influencers derives from perceived similarity, rather than wishful identification.56 Influencers are able to mimic parasocial interaction which strengthens bonds with their followers, whereas traditional celebrities do not tend to interact with their fans online.57 Schouten et al.33 found that consumers felt higher degrees of wishful identification with influencers, rather than celebrities, and explained this as people aspiring to become social media influencers nowadays;58 however, this finding was within low involvement goods, and consumers may be sceptical that celebrities actually use these cheaper products as they do not find them as congruent with the match-up hypothesis. Luxury products are high involvement goods, as well as aspirational, and seeing that consumers feel wishful identification with celebrities, it can be hypothesised that:

HP1: Consumers wishfully identify more with the celebrity endorser than social media influencer endorser when evaluating the luxury goods advertisement.

Actual self-congruity

Self-congruity theory is the concept that consumers use brands for the practical and symbolic uses they have pertaining to the consumers’ own self-concept, and that self-congruity is the match-up between a brand and a person’s view of themselves.59,60 Self-congruity is the idea that the more alike the brand and a person’s concepts are, the higher the desire for the brand, as the brand supports the person’s identity.61 Luxury consumption literature shows that consumers buy luxury products to reflect self-concepts.62 Achieving congruence between the different elements of one’s self-concept and brand image is important for validating one’s feelings of satisfaction with self and social consistency, self-esteem and social approval needs.63,64 When a brand’s product-user image is congruent with a consumer’s self-concept, it results in higher product and brand attitudes.65,66 Younger consumers look for products which match their self-concept; therefore, if an endorser matches their self-image, consumers are more likely to positively engage with a brand.67,68 When consumers believe that they are similar to an endorser, they are more likely to follow their opinions.69,70 Influencers are perceived as more relatable which promotes an easier identification process.33 This has been found with luxury handbags and YouTube Vloggers.71 Celebrities are considered as socially distant,72 whereas influencers portray themselves as being more like regular people.33 Consumers believe they have more in common with influencers, therefore:

HP2: Consumers’ perceived actual self-congruity will be stronger in the context of the social media influencer endorsement rather than the celebrity endorsement.

Ideal social self-congruity

People aspire to their ideal self-image so they can extend their true self-image.73 As luxury is symbolic and people purchase luxury goods due to social motivations, an understanding of the ideal social self is a necessity as the influence of meeting one’s ideal social self is more pronounced with these goods.65 The consumption of luxury is motivated through peers and social values.19 As luxury goods are not a basic need, they are purchased as a means to enhance one’s ideal self-image.74 Luxury brands have the ability to improve the user’s self-concept and worth, which is not always possible with non-luxury brands.71 Luxury brands’ advertising is designed to appeal to consumers’ ideal self-image as a way of portraying the aspirational nature of the brand.75,76

Within the context of luxury, celebrity endorsements have been shown to be valuable. Okonkwo77 theorised that celebrity endorsements held long-term benefits for luxury brands, such as increased brand loyalty and brand equity, which translates to a higher sales turnover. Celebrity endorsement of luxury goods has been found to influence purchasing decisions,78,79 whereas Sharma34 found that celebrities can motivate consumers to purchase luxury products if they trust the value of the brand. Through match-up hypothesis, consumers’ perceptions transfer from the endorser to the product, e.g. premium price with high-class celebrity.80

Thus, it can be hypothesised that:

HP3: Celebrities encourage stronger positive attitudes towards luxury advertisements compared to social media influencers; and

HP4: Celebrities are more effective endorsers for luxury goods than social media influencers.

Methodology

Survey Measures

Ideal social self was measured with Han and Hyun’s81 ideal social-self congruity scale, adapted to include ‘handbag’ (α 0.862). Product perception was measured using Harben and Kim’s82 scale of liking for clothing products (α 0.84). Using a scale adapted from Spears and Singh83 (α 0.97), purchase intention toward the handbag product was measured. Actual-self congruity was measured using a scale adapted to the current context, from Sirgy and Su84 (α 0.83). The survey measured wishful identification using Hoffner and Buchanan’s52 scale originally developed for television characters (α 0.84 for female characters). Attitude toward the advertisement was assessed using a scale based on Spears and Singh’s83 study, adapted to the context of the current study. Scale items are shown in Tables 1–8.

Stimulus selection and pre-test

A pre-test was conducted to examine which brands, influencers and celebrities would serve as appropriate stimuli for the experiment. A convenience sample of 10 female students at the University of Otago was recruited for the pre-test, and they were presented with pictures of three influencer endorsers, three celebrity endorsers and three luxury handbags. Three handbag brands were selected for the pre-test (Louis Vuitton; Gucci; Yves St Laurent) all of whom target young consumers, and all have used social media campaigns. Similar, entry price-point bags were selected for inclusion. The influencers and celebrities used in the pre-test were selected for age (under 25 years, to ensure social relevance to the participants) and high recognisability among the target population. Further, all endorsers had previously worked with luxury brands. In the pre-test, participants were asked to indicate how well they identified with the endorsers and how recognisable they perceived endorsers to be. The requirement for recognisability was established as an experimental control to minimise the effects of unfamiliarity as a confounding variable. For the main experiment, the celebrity and the influencer with the highest fit score was selected (International actress Sophie Turner for the celebrity and New Zealand social media influencer Jamie Ridge). The Yves Saint Laurent ‘Kate’ bag was selected as the most desirable handbag to the pre-test group.

Sample

The study used a non-probability convenience sample of women between 18-25 years, recruited online, via social media. The age cohort for the sample was based on prior research that examines the impact of celebrity endorsements, which finds that this approach is most useful in targeting young consumers.85 Further, studies show that consumers in this age cohort are increasingly investing in luxury goods.86–88 Women are said to be more predominant users of social media and more likely to engage in conspicuous consumption than men.89,90 Participation in the study was incentivised with the chance to win one of two $100 NZD gift cards. 153 participants took part in the study, consistent with extant literature.33,38

Procedure

In the study, participants were randomly assigned to either treatment group (A) the celebrity condition, or treatment group (B), the influencer condition. The experiment was hosted via Qualtrics, and participants first saw a photograph of the luxury product (a handbag) and were then asked to respond to 7-point Likert scales for items measuring ideal social self-congruity, product perception and purchase intention. In the second part of the study participants saw a manipulated image of either the (A) celebrity or (B) influencer endorser, alongside the product and the brand logo and again asked to respond to 7-point Likert scales for items measuring the perceived level of expertise of the endorser, actual self-congruity, wishful identification, attitude towards the advertisement and purchase intention.

Data analysis

153 survey responses were collected and exported to Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) for analysis. Inspecting the data, six cases were removed due to a non-response error. Factor analysis, comparison of means and one-way ANOVA were used to analyse the data.

Validation of scales

All scales had Cronbach alphas of over 0.8, indicating acceptable reliability within this study.91 All scales had Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) values of above the threshold of 0.6 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001, cited in Shepherd & Edelmann,92) and the Eigenvalues indicated there was one dominant factor in all scales (ideal social self-congruity: 2.298, product perception: 2.616, expertise: 3.289, actual self-congruity: 3.366, wishful identification: 2.963, attitude towards advertisement: 4.177). A principal factor analyses further confirmed that all values loaded onto one component, and as such, the scales can be seen to be reliable.

Results

Pre-endorser exposure

The stimulus utilised was liked and considered stylish, indicating its appropriateness for measuring the effect of endorsers, however purchase intention was towards the low end of the scale (Table 1). Participants who did wish to buy the handbag felt that their ideal self was more similar to the typical owner of a luxury handbag in comparison to those who would not like to purchase the handbag ((m = 4.14) (p = 0.000), (m = 4.95) (p = 0.000), (m = 4.65) (p = 0.000)) (Table 2).

Those who wanted to buy the handbag had higher overall product perceptions ((m = 6.08) (p = 0.000), (m = 5.84) (p = 0.000), (m = 6.03) (p = 0.000)) (Table 3) and those who did wish to buy the handbag felt that the endorser was ‘similar to how I am’ (m = 3.14) (p = 0.002). The remaining items on the scale were non-significant (Table 4).

Those who would like to buy the handbag had higher levels of wishful identification (m = 4.22) (p = 0.035) and emulation (m = 4.38) (p = 0.031); however, the other items within the scale were non-significant (Table 5).

Celebrity and influencer condition treatment comparison

Treatment group A (Celebrity) expressed more positive attitudes towards the advertisement than did Treatment group B (Influencer) (Table 6).

Participants who were exposed to the celebrity advertisement expressed greater agreement with statements that the endorser was ‘similar to how I see myself’ (m = 2.75) (p = 0.010) and ‘similar to how others believe that I am’ (m = 2.63) (p = 0.016) (Table 7). Participants also felt greater levels of wishful identification towards celebrities in most items of the scale ((m = 4.18) (p = 0.000), (m = 4.56) (p = 0.010), (m = 4.27) (p = 0.001)) (Table 8) (Table 9).

Ideal social self-congruity

Owns a luxury bag

Does not own a luxury bag

 

N=34

N=113

 

The typical owner of this handbag:

Mean

Mean

Sig.

Has personality characteristics similar to mine, as perceived by others

3.94

2.81

.000**

Has an image similar to how I would like other people to see me

4.47

3.31

.000**

Is very much the kind of person I would like other people to see me as

4.09

3.1

.001**

Product perception

The handbag in this image is stylish

5.62

5.28

0.199

The handbag in this image is attractive

5.62

5.07

.048*

Overall, I like the handbag in this image

5.59

5.07

0.07

Purchase intention

How likely are you to buy this handbag?

4.24

2.98

.000**

Table 1 Product stimulus congruency & perceptions
1=Strongly disagree, 7=Strongly agree, 1=Definitely not buy, 7=Definitely buy.

The typical owner of this handbag:

Definitely would purchase

Definitely would not purchase

Total

Sig.

N=37

N=81

N=118

Mean

Mean

Mean

Has personality characteristics similar to mine, as perceived by others

4.14

2.54

3.04

.000**

Has an image similar to how I would like other people to see me

4.95

2.88

3.53

.000**

Is very much the kind of person I would like other people to see me as

4.65

2.63

3.26

.000**

Table 2 Ideal social self-congruity by purchase intention
*Significant at 0.05. 1=Strongly disagree, 7=Strongly agree.

For the following statements, please select a response

Definitely would purchase

Definitely would not purchase

Total

Sig.

N=37

N=81

N=118

Mean

Mean

Mean

The handbag in this image is stylish

6.08

4.86

5.25

.000**

The handbag in this image is attractive

5.84

4.72

5.07

.000**

Overall, I like the handbag in this image

6.03

4.58

5.03

.000**

Table 3 Product perception by purchase intention
*Significant at 0.05. 1=Strongly disagree, 7= Strongly agree.

The woman in this advertisement is:

Definitely would purchase

Definitely would not purchase

Total

Sig.

N=37

N=81

N=118

Mean

Mean

Mean

Similar to how I am

3.14

2.4

2.63

.002*

Similar to how I see myself

2.7

2.35

2.46

0.14

Similar to how others believe that I am

2.59

2.26

2.36

0.183

Similar to how others see me

2.84

2.35

2.53

0.094

Table 4 Actual self-congruity by purchase intention
*Significant at 0.05. 1= Strongly disagree, 7=Strongly agree.

For the following statements, please select a response.

Definitely would purchase

Definitely would not purchase

Total

Sig.

N=37

N=81

N=118

Mean

Mean

Mean

The type of person who would own this handbag is the type of person I want to be like

4.22

3.53

3.75

.035*

Sometimes I wish I could be more like the type of person who would own this handbag

4.62

4.22

4.35

0.193

The type of person who would own this handbag is someone I would like to emulate

4.38

3.69

3.91

.031*

I’d like to do the kind of things the type of person who owns this handbag would

4.32

3.72

3.93

0.08

Table 5 Wishful identification by purchase intention
*Significant at 0.05. 1=Strongly disagree, 7=Strongly agree.

I find this promotional image:

Treatment A (Celebrity)

Treatment B (Influencer)

Sig.

N=73

N=74

Unappealing/Appealing

4.74

3.97

.002*

Bad/Good

4.75

4

.002*

Unpleasant/Pleasant

4.93

4.19

.001**

Unfavourable/Favourable

4.75

3.96

.000**

Unlikable/likeable

4.93

4.03

.000**

Table 6 Attitudes toward advertisement
*Significant at 0.05; Semantic differential scale of 1–7 for every item.

The woman in this advertisement is:

Treatment A (Celebrity) N=73

Treatment B (Influencer) N=74

Sig.

Similar to how I am

2.82

2.5

0.09

Similar to how I see myself

2.75

2.2

.010*

Similar to how others believe that I am

2.63

2.14

.016*

Similar to how others see me

2.62

2.49

0.489

Table 7 Actual self-congruity with endorser type
*Significant at 0.05; 1=Strongly disagree, 7=Strongly agree.

Considering the advert:

Treatment A (Celebrity) N=73

Treatment B (Influencer) N=74

Sig.

The woman in this image is the type of person I want myself to be like

4.18

3.26

.000**

Sometimes I wish I could be more like the woman in this image

4.56

3.92

.010*

The woman in this image is someone I would like to emulate

4.27

3.43

.001**

I’d like to do the kind of things the woman in this image does

4.04

3.92

0.802

Table 8 Wishful identification with endorser type
*Significant at 0.05. 1=Strongly disagree, 7=Strongly agree.

Hypotheses

Outcome

HP1: Consumers wishfully identify more with the celebrity endorser than social media influencer endorser when evaluating the luxury goods advertisement.

Supported

HP3: Consumers’ perceived actual self-congruity will be stronger in the context of the social media influencer endorsement rather than the celebrity endorsement.

Not supported

HP4: Celebrities encourage stronger positive attitudes towards luxury advertisements compared to social media influencers.

Supported

HP5: Celebrities are more effective endorsers for luxury goods than social media influencers.

Supported

Table 9 Hypotheses summary

Discussion

This study contributes by directly comparing the endorsement of luxury products by celebrities and social media influencers, in evaluating their effectiveness as endorsers. The study sought to understand how young adult female consumers related to and perceived endorsers in luxury advertising, and which endorser was more effective in promoting positive attitudes toward the advertisement, as well as stimulating purchase intention. Overall it was found that participants felt their actual self was more similar to the celebrity, they wishfully identified with the celebrity more, and had a more positive response to the celebrity advertisement. This indicates that the celebrity condition was supported by the match-up hypothesis,93 which finds celebrities as a more effective match with luxury goods than social media influencers. It was found that the celebrity condition reinforced the existing associations of luxury.

The match-up hypothesis

The first research question asked whether consumers engaged more with celebrities or social media endorsers when evaluating luxury product advertisements. This study found that participants had higher levels of wishful identification with celebrities, supporting H1. This contrasts Schouten et al.33’s findings, that consumers wishfully identified with influencers more than celebrities. However, Schouten et al.33 used low involvement goods as their stimulus (e.g. makeup/ protein shakes), as opposed to luxury goods. The pairing of social media influencers and low involvement goods may be more convincing, as influencers, these seemingly regular people, are perceived as more likely to use these products. It may be more believable that celebrities use luxury goods, due to the match-up hypothesis placing them in the same category of aspiration. Here attribution theory can be applied, as the fee involved in employing an endorser affects consumer perception (Silvera & Austad, 2004). In evaluating an advertisement, consumers judge the internal attribution, whereby they believe that the endorser promotes a product because they personally believe in its value, and external attribution, whereby they believe the endorser promotes the product because they are being paid (Hsu & McDonald, 2002).

Self-concept and endorsement

The majority of participants did not greatly aspire to be like the typical owner of the luxury handbag, before the introduction of the endorser. The handbag alone did not encourage strong aspirations for meeting one’s ideal social self, and as such, demonstrates the value of including a person in advertisements to illustrate perceived social benefits and promote identification. H2 was not supported, as participants self-concept was aligned more with the celebrity than the influencer. This is a new finding within social media influencer research, where prior studies align actual self and influencers (Chapple & Cownie, 2017).32,33,56

Attitudes to advertisement

The second research question asked whether luxury brands would benefit from celebrity or social media influencers as endorsers, in regard to improved purchase intention. H4, that celebrities encourage stronger positive attitudes towards luxury advertisements compared to social media influencers was supported. This is consistent with existing theory38,43,44,94 which suggests the combination of celebrity endorsers and luxury leads consumers to have more positive perceptions about an advertisement.

Purchase intention

Neither treatment had a significant effect on purchase intention. Extant theory is mixed, with studies indicating that celebrity endorsement can significantly, and not significantly, influence purchase intention. Daneschvary & Schwer78 and Gardner & Shuman79 found that celebrity endorsement positively influenced purchase intention. Yet, as these studies are dated, it could signify that young consumers are becoming less responsive to celebrity endorsements (Kirkpatrick, 2017), and by extension, less receptive to advertising (Newman, 2015). Erdogen37 theorised that a positive reaction to a celebrity would not necessarily lead to a change in behaviour or a purchase decision and Park and Yim38 note that celebrities do not drive purchase intention when evaluating luxury goods. While this study’s participants had more positive attitudes to the celebrity advertisement, the inclusion of the celebrity was not motivating enough to encourage purchase intention. This could be explained as a function of the cost of a luxury bag, as although consumers like the endorser and the handbag, the advertisement does not motivate them to consider purchasing.

While only a small proportion of the sample (23.1%) owned a luxury bag, these participants also had significantly higher levels of ideal social self-congruity. They also found the bag more attractive and perceived their actual self-congruity as more similar to the endorser that they were presented with. As brand identification leads to brand loyalty (Hongwei et. al., 2012), consumers who are already in the market identify more with luxury brands, resulting in higher purchase intention. This is supported by the literature, as attitudinal brand loyalty has been found to be high within luxury goods (Thakur, 2015). As luxury consumers have an emotional relationship with luxury brands, it can predict purchase intention (Bain & Co. 2005; Choo et al. 2012). The handbag owners group had a more positive reception to the advertisement. Supporting the match-up hypothesis,95 where the consumer who finds their identity similar to the brand has positive purchase intentions and attitudes towards the advertisement.96 Handbag owners had significantly higher purchase intentions before and after the exposure to the endorser advertisement. This suggests that once people already belong to the target market, they are more receptive towards the advertisement. However, to a degree this signifies that it does not matter who is endorsing the product. This is a significant finding in this study.97–120

Conclusion

This study sought to examine whether celebrities or social media influencers were more effective in endorsing luxury brands. Self-congruity theory and match-up hypothesis were used to analyse the effects of the endorser advertisement. Despite the increase in luxury brands utilising social media influencers, there is little literature on the effectiveness of influencers within the luxury context, and even less comparing the traditional luxury endorser of the celebrity to social media influencers. This study sought to fill this gap in the literature and to better understand the role of endorsers in luxury advertising by conducting an experiment to test how consumers feel about both endorsers. The results of this study indicate that consumers favoured the celebrity condition; however, this was not a particularly strong result. As such, it widens the scope of this study, and asks if endorsement is really necessary for luxury brands. Endorsement is an expensive and high-risk venture, and this study places a question mark over its value in luxury marketing.

While this study found that luxury handbag ownership played a significant role in the analysing of data, people’s personal opinions of what constitutes a luxury handbag may differ. This study considers a luxury handbag to be in line with the accessible core and above of Rambourg’s (2014) Pyramid of Luxury (Appendix I), which some participants may have incorrectly reported. Further, whilst the study design featured mock advertisements, usually these are placed on social media as part of a larger campaign (Kapitan & Silvera, 2016) and as the study does not mimic this it loses external validity when discussing the findings in a more general manner. While this study compares influencers and celebrities, the lines between these two are becoming increasingly blurred, as celebrities often have large followings on social media platforms, and influencers are becoming so well-known they are becoming celebrities.

Appendix 1

Acknowledgments

None.

Funding

None.

Conflicts of interest

Author declares that there is no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kapferer JN, Laurent G. Where do consumers think luxury begins? A study of perceived minimum price for 21 luxury goods in 7 countries. Journal of Business Research. 2014;69(1):1–10.
  2. Freire NA. When luxury advertising adds the identity values of luxury: A semiotic analysis. Journal of Business Research. 2014;67(12):2666–2675.
  3. Kastanakis MN, Balabanis G. Between the mass and the class: Antecedents of the ‘bandwagon’ luxury consumption behaviour. Journal of Business Research. 2012;65:1399–1407.
  4. Jiang M, Gao D‐G, Huang R, et al. Luxury brands. Asian Journal of Social Psychology. 2014;17:245–254.
  5. McFerran B, Aquino K, Tracy JL. Evidence for two facets of pride in consumption: Findings from luxury brands. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 2014;24(4):311–322.
  6. Kapferer JN, Bastien V. The luxury strategy: Break the rules of marketing to build luxury brands. London: Kogan Page Limited; 2009.
  7. Dion D, Arnould E. Retail luxury strategy: Assembling charisma through art and magic. Journal of Retailing. 2011;(4):502–520.
  8. Kapferer JN. The new strategic brand management: Advanced insights and strategic thinking. Great Britain: Kogan Page Publishers; 2012.
  9. Kim A, Ko E. Impacts of luxury fashion brand’s social media marketing on customer relationship and purchase intention. Journal of Global Fashion Marketing. 2010;1:164–171.
  10. Kim A, Ko E. Do social media marketing activities enhance customer equity? An empirical study of luxury fashion brand. Journal of Business Research. 2012;65(10):1480–1486.
  11. Hughes MU, Bendoni W, Pehlivan E. Storygiving as a co–creation tool for luxury brands in the age of the internet: A love story by Tiffany and thousands of lovers. Journal of Product & Brand Management. 2016;25(4):357–364.
  12. Godey B, Manthiou A, Pederzoli D, et al. Social media marketing efforts of luxury brands: Influence on brand equity and consumer behaviour. Journal of Business Research. 2016;69(12):5833–5841.
  13. Miyazaki AD, Grewal D, Goodstein RC. The effect of multiple extrinsic cues on quality perceptions: A matter of consistency. Journal of Consumer Research. 2005;32:146–153.
  14. Silverstein MJ, Fiske N. Luxury for the masses. Harvard Business Review. 2003.
  15. Douglas M, Isherwood B. The world of goods. New York: Basic Books; 1979.
  16. Batra R, Ramaswamy V, Alden DL, et al. Effects of brand local and nonlocal origin on consumer attitudes in developing countries. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 2000;9:83–95.
  17. Akram A, Merunka D, Akram MS. Perceived brand globalness in emerging markets and the moderating role of consumer ethnocentrism. International Journal of Emerging Markets. 2011;6(4):291–303.
  18. Tan MZ, Teoh SY, Tan CE, et al. Perceived purchase intention of undergraduates towards luxury brands: Case study. Proceeding of the International Conference on Social Science Research, ICSSR; 2013. e–ISBN 978–967–11768–1–8.
  19. Vigneron F, Johnson L. Measuring perception of brand luxury. Journal of Brand Management. 2004;11(6):484–506.
  20. Wilcox K, Hyeong MK, Sen S. Why do consumers buy counterfeit luxury brands?. Journal of Marketing Research. 2009;46:247–259.
  21. Han YJ, Nunes JC, Drèze X. Signaling status with luxury goods: The role of brand prominence. Journal of Marketing. 2010;74:15–30.
  22. Fuchs C, Prandelli E, Schreier M, et al. All that is users might not be gold: How labeling products as user designed backfires in the context of luxury fashion brands. Journal of Marketing. 2013;77(5):75–91.
  23. Roy R, Rabbanee FK. Antecedents and consequences of self–congruity. European Journal of Marketing. 2015;49(3/4):444–466.
  24. Belk RW, Bahn KD, Mayer RN. Developmental recognition of consumption symbolism. Journal of Consumer Research. 1982;9(1):4–17.
  25. Mead NL, Baumeister RF, Stillman TF, et al. Social exclusion causes people to spend and consume strategically in the service of affiliation. Journal of Consumer Research. 2011;37(5):902–919.
  26. Fernandes AR. Understanding influencer endorsement in the luxury sector. (Unpublished dissertation). Lisbon, Portugal: Universidade Católica Portuguesa; 2018.
  27. Radon A. Communicating luxury brand exclusivity online. (Unpublished). 2012.
  28. Proctor T, Kitchen PJ. Celebrity ambassador/celebrity endorsement – takes a licking but keeps on ticking. Journal of Strategic Marketing. 2019;27(5):373–387.
  29. Lueck JA. Friend–zone with benefits: The parasocial advertising of Kim Kardashian. Journal of Marketing Communications. 2015;21(2):91–109.
  30. Friedman H, Santeramo J, Anthony T. Correlates of trustworthiness for celebrities. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science. 1979;6(4):291–299.
  31. Nouri M. The power of influence: Traditional celebrity vs. social media influencer. Advanced Writing: Pop Culture Intersections. 2018;32:1–20.
  32. Djafarova E, Rushworth C. Exploring the credibility of online celebrities’ Instagram profiles in influencing the purchase decisions of young female users. Computers in Human Behavior. 2017;68:1–7.
  33. Schouten AP, Loes Janssen L, Verspaget M. Celebrity vs. influencer endorsements in advertising: The role of identification, credibility, and product endorser fit. International Journal of Advertising. 2019:1–25.
  34. Sharma R. Effect of celebrity endorsements on brand quality perceptions & brand loyalty – a comparative study of luxury & non luxury brands in India. AIMA Journal of Management & Research. 2015;9(4/4):1–13.
  35. Sims J. From Cara to Clooney, the lucrative art of celebrity watch endorsement. CNN. 2016.
  36. Daboll P. Celebrities in advertising are almost always a big waste of money. Advertising Age. 2010.
  37. Erdogan BZ. Celebrity endorsement: A literature review. Journal of Marketing Management. 1999;15(4):291–314.
  38. Park S–Y, Yim M Y–C. Do celebrity endorsements benefit familiar luxury brands? A perspective from social adaptation theory. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising. 2018:1–16.
  39. Belch GE, Belch MA. Advertising and promotion: An integrated marketing communications perspective. 5th edn. New York: McGraw Hill Irwin; 2012.
  40. Aaker DA, Carmen JM. Are you over advertising? Journal of Advertising Research. 1982;22(4):57–70.
  41. Tellis GJ. Effective advertising: How, when and why advertising works. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2004.
  42. Nelson O, Deborah A. Celebrity endorsement influence on brand credibility: A critical review of previous studies. Online Journal of Communication and Media Technologies. 2017;7(1):15–32.  
  43. Atkins C, Block M. Effectiveness of celebrity endorsers. Journal of Advertising Research. 1983;23(1):57–61.
  44. Petty RE, Cacioppo JT, Schumann D. Central and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: The moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Research. 1983;10(2):135–146.
  45. Evans NJ, Phua J, Lim J, et al. Disclosing Instagram influencer advertising: The effects of disclosure language on advertising recognition, attitudes, and behavioral intent. Journal of Interactive Advertising. 2017;17(2):138–149.
  46. Basil MD. Identification as a mediator of celebrity effects. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media. 1996;40:478–95.
  47. Ohanian R. Construction and validation of a scale to measure celebrity endorsers’ perceived expertise, trustworthiness, and attractiveness. Journal of Advertising. 1990;19(3):39–52.
  48. Cohen J. Defining identification: A theoretical look at the identification of audiences with media characters. Mass Communication & Society. 2001;4(3):245–264.
  49. Kamins MA, Brand MJ, Hoeke SA, et al. Two–sided versus one–sided celebrity endorsements: The impact on advertising effectiveness and credibility. Journal of Advertising. 1989;18(2):4–10.
  50. Bandura A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice–Hall, Inc; 1986.
  51. Moyer–Gusé E. Toward a theory of entertainment persuasion: Explaining the persuasive effects of entertainment–education messages. Communication Theory. 2008;18:407–425.
  52. Hoffner C, Buchanan M. Young adults’ wishful identification with television characters: The role of perceived similarity and character attributes. Media Psychology. 2005;7(4):325–351.
  53. Konijn EA, Nije Bijvank M, Bushman BJ. I wish I were a warrior: Effects of violent video games on adolescent boys. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43(4):1038–1044.
  54. Shoenberger K, Kim EA. Product placement as leveraged marketing communications: The role of wishful identification, brand trust, and brand buying behaviours. International Journal of Advertising. 2019;38(1):50–66.
  55. Parmar B, Patel PR. Fame versus no name: Gauging the impact of celebrities and non–celebrities’ endorsement on purchase. African Journal of Business Management. 2015;9:127–133.
  56. Gräve JF. Exploring the perception of influencers vs. traditional celebrities: Are social media stars a new type of endorser? In Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Social Media & Society. ACM. 2017. 36 p.
  57. Labrecque LI. Fostering consumer–brand relationships in social media environments: The role of parasocial interaction. Journal of Interactive Marketing. 2014;28(2):134–148.
  58. Chae J. Explaining females’ envy toward social media influencers. Media Psychology. 2018;21(2):246–262.
  59. Sirgy J. Self–concept in relation to product preference intention. Developments in Marketing Science. 1980;3:350–354.
  60. Sirgy MJ. Self–concept in consumer behavior: A critical review. Journal of Consumer Research. 1982;9:287–300.
  61. Swann WB, Stein–Seroussi A, Giesler BR. Why people self–verify. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1992;62(3):392–401.
  62. Randhawa P, Calatone RJ, Voorhees CM. The pursuit of counterfeited luxury: An examination of the negative side effects of close consumer–brand connections. Journal of Business Research. 2015;68(11):2395–2403.
  63. Aguirre–Rodriguez A, Bosnjak M, Sirgy MJ. Moderators of the self–congruity effect on consumer decision–making: A meta–analysis. Journal of Business Research. 2012;65(8):1179–1188.
  64. Hosany S, Martin D. Self–image congruence in consumer behaviour. Journal of Business Research. 2012;65(5):685–691.
  65. Liu F, Li J, Mizerski D, et al. Self–congruity, brand attitude, and brand loyalty: A study on luxury brands. European Journal of Marketing. 2012;46(7/8):922–937.
  66. Johar JS, Sirgy JM. Value–expressive versus utilitarian advertising appeals: When and why to use which appeal. Journal of Advertising. 1991;20(3):23–33.
  67. Lippe D. It’s all in creative delivery. Advertising Age. 2001;72(26):S8–S9.
  68. McCormick K. Celebrity endorsements: Influence of a product–endorser match on millennials’ attitudes and purchase intentions. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services. 2016;32:39–45.
  69. Cialdini R. The psychology of influence. New York: William Morrow & Co; 1993.
  70. Kelman HC. Interests, relationships, identities: Three central issues for individuals and groups in negotiating their social environment. Annual Review of Psychology. 2006;57(1):1–26.
  71. Lee JE, Watkins B. YouTube vloggers’ influence on consumer luxury brand perceptions and intentions. Journal of Business Research. 2016;69(12):5753–5760.
  72. Hansen J, Wänke M. The abstractness of luxury. Journal of Economic Psychology. 2011;32(5):789–796.
  73. Wang S, Hsu C, Huang H, et al. How readers’ perceived self–congruity and functional congruity affect bloggers’ informational influence: Perceived interactivity as a moderator. Online Information Review. 2015;39(4):537–555.
  74. Correia Loureiro SM, Maximiano M, Panchapakesan P. Engaging fashion consumers in social media: The case of luxury brands. International Journal of Fashion Design, Technology and Education. 2018;11(3):310–321.
  75. Lee SH, Workman JE. Vanity, fashion leadership, and self–consciousness among South Korean male and female college students. International Journal of Fashion Design, Technology and Education. 2014;7(2):115–124.
  76. Malär L, Krohmer H, Hoyer WD, et al. Emotional brand attachment and brand personality: The relative importance of the actual and the ideal self. Journal of Marketing. 2011;75:35–52.
  77. Okonkwo U. Luxury brands & celebrities: An enduring branding romance white paper. 2006.
  78. Daneshvary R, Schwer KR. The association endorsement and consumers’ intention to purchase. Journal of Consumer Marketing. 2000;17(3):203–213.
  79. Gardner MP, Shuman PJ. Sponsorship: An important component of the promotions mix. Journal of Advertising. 1986;16(1):11–17.
  80. Miller FM, Allen CT. How does celebrity meaning transfer? Investigating the process of meaning transfer with celebrity affiliates and mature brands. Journal of Consumer Psychology. 2012;22:443–452.
  81. Han H, Hyun S. Image congruence and relationship quality in predicting switching intention: Conspicuousness of product use as a moderator variable. Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research. 2012;37:303–329.
  82. Harben B, Kim S. Attitude towards fashion advertisements with political content: Impacts of opinion leadership and perception of advertisement message. International Journal of Consumer Studies. 2008;32:88–98.
  83. Spears N, Singh SN. Measuring attitude toward the brand and purchase intentions. Journal of Current Issues & Research in Advertising. 2004;26:53–66.
  84. Sirgy M, Su C. Destination image, self–congruity, and travel behavior: Toward an integrative model. Journal of Travel Research. 2000;38:340–352.
  85. Martin C, Bush A. Do role models influence teenagers’ purchase intentions and behaviour?. Journal of Consumer Marketing. 2000;17(5):441–453.
  86. Twitchell JB. Living it up: Our love affair with luxury. New York, NY: Columbia University Press; 2002.
  87. Arienti P. Global powers of luxury goods 2019. Deloitte. 2019.
  88. Chadha R, Husband P. The cult of luxury brands: Inside Asia’s love affair with luxury. London, Boston: Nicholas Brealey International; 2006.
  89. Sheldon P, Bryant K. Instagram: Motives for its use and relationship to narcissism and contextual age. Computers in Human Behavior. 2016;5:89–97.
  90. O’Cass A, McEwen H. Exploring consumer status and conspicuous consumption. Journal of Consumer Behaviour. 2004;4(1):25–39.
  91. Malhotra NK, Birks D F. Marketing research: An applied approach. Harlow: Pearson Education; 2006.
  92. Shepherd RM, Edelmann RJ. Reasons for internet use and social anxiety. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005;39(5):949–958.
  93. Kamins MA. An investigation into the ‘match–up’ hypothesis in celebrity advertising: When beauty may be only skin deep. Journal of Advertising. 1990;19(1):4–13.
  94. Kahle LN, Homer PM. Physical attractiveness of the celebrity endorser: A social adaptation perspective. Journal of Consumer Research. 1985;11:954–961.
  95. Till BD, Busler M. The match–up hypothesis: Physical attractiveness, expertise, and the role of fit on brand attitude, purchase intent and brand beliefs. Journal of Advertising. 2000;29(3):1–13.
  96. Choi SM, Rifon NJ. It is a match: The impact of congruence between celebrity image and consumer ideal self on endorsement effectiveness. Psychology & Marketing. 2012;29:639–650.
  97. Arrigo E. Social media opportunities for market–driven firms. In: Lee I, editor. Integrating Social Media into Business Practice, Applications, Management, and Models. Hershey, USA: IGI Global; 2014:180–199.
  98. Atwal G, Williams A. Advances in luxury brand management. In: Kapferer JN, Kernstock J, Brexendorf TO, Powell SM, editors. Luxury brand marketing – the experience is everything. Cham, Switzerland: Palgrave Macmillan; 2009:43–57.
  99. Barnier VD, Valette–Florence P. Culture and luxury: an analysis of luxury perceptions across frontiers. In: Hennigs N, Wiedmann KP, editors. Luxury Marketing. Hannover, Germany: Gabler Verlag; 2013:37–56.  
  100. Castronovo C, Huang L. Social media in an alternative marketing communication model. Journal of Marketing Development and Competitiveness. 2012;6(1):117–134.
  101. Chevalier M, Gutsatz M. Luxury retail management: How the world’s top brands provide quality product and service support. Singapore: John Wiley & Sons; 2012.
  102. Dubois B, Paternault C. Observations – understanding the world of international luxury brands: The dream formula. Journal of Advertising Research. 1995;35(4):69–76.
  103. Gush C. Dior's saddle bag is back – along with a controversial influencer campaign. 2018.
  104. Hennigs N, Wiedmann KP, Klarmann C. Luxury brands in the digital age – exclusivity versus ubiquity. Marketing Review. 2012;29(1):30–35.
  105. Jackson T, Haid C. Gucci group – the new family of luxury brands. International Journal of New Product Development and Innovation Management. 2002;4(2):161–172.
  106. Kamins MA, Gupta K. Congruence between spokesperson and product type: A matchup hypothesis perspective. Psychology & Marketing. 1994;1(6):569–586.
  107. Klein AV. Can mass influencer marketing backfire for brands?. Fashionista. 2018.
  108. Nueno JL, Quelch JA. The mass marketing of luxury. Business Horizons. 1998;41(6):61–68.
  109. Okonkwo U. Sustaining the luxury brand on the Internet. Journal of Brand Management. 2009;16(5–6):302–310.
  110. Parrott G, Danbury A, Kanthavanich P. Online behaviour of luxury fashion brand advocates. Journal of Fashion Marketing and Management. 2015;19(4):360–383.
  111. Phan M, Thomas R, Heine K. Social media and luxury brand management: The case of Burberry. Journal of Global Fashion Marketing. 2011;2:213–222.
  112. Phau I, Prendergast G. Conceptualizing the country of origin of brand. Journal of Marketing Communications. 2000;6:159–170.
  113. Rossitier JR, Percy L. Attitude change through visual imagery in advertising. Journal of Advertising Research. 1980;9(2):10–16.
  114. Saeed R, Zameer H, Naseer R, et al. Impact of celebrity and non–celebrity advertisement on consumer perception. Applied Sciences and Business Economics. 2014;1(3):51–56.
  115. Schlecht C. Celebrities’ impact on branding. Center on Global Brand Leadership. New York: Colombia Business School; 2003.
  116. Seno D, Lukas BA. The equity effect of product endorsement by celebrities: A conceptual framework from a co–branding perspective. European Journal of Marketing. 2007;41(1/2):121–134.
  117. Steel E. To reach the truly rich, television ads won’t do. The New York Times. 2015.
  118. Technovia. Global luxury handbags market 2019–2023. 2019.
  119. Till BD. Using celebrity endorsers effectively: Lessons from associative learning. The Journal of Product and Brand Management. 1998;7(5):400.
  120. Till B, Shimp T. Endorsers in advertising: The case of negative celebrity information. Journal of Advertising. 1998;27(1):67–82.
Creative Commons Attribution License

©2024 McNeill, et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and build upon your work non-commercially.