Submit manuscript...
eISSN: 2378-315X

Biometrics & Biostatistics International Journal

Research Article Volume 5 Issue 4

Growth in reading comprehension and mathematics achievement in primary school: a bivariate transition multilevel growth curve model approach

Dickson Nkafu Anumendem,1 Bieke De Fraine,1 Patrick Onghena,2 Jan Van Damme3

1Centre for Educational Effectiveness and Evaluation of the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Belgium
2Centre for Methodology in Educational Sciences of the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Belgium
3Centre for Educational Effectiveness and Evaluation, both at the Katholieke Universiteit, Belgium

Correspondence: Nkafu Dickson Anumendem, Centre for Educational Effectiveness and Evaluation of the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven, Dekenstraat 2 / 3773, 3000 Leuven, Belgium, Tel 32-163-257-75

Received: August 08, 2016 | Published: March 28, 2017

Citation: Anumendem DN. Growth in reading comprehension and mathematics achievement in primary school: a bivariate transition multilevel growth curve model approach. Biom Biostat Int J. 2017;5(4):112-123. DOI: 10.15406/bbij.2017.05.00137

Download PDF

Introducton

The measurement and explanation of school effects on both students’ reading comprehension and mathematics achievement in primary school children has seldom been studied. Some studies have focused on mathematics achievement as a predictor of reading comprehension1 or reading as a predictor of mathematics performance.2 These studies make the key assumption of deciding in advance which of the outcomes is dependent on the other. A few multivariate studies have nonetheless been done to investigate the possibility of a relationship between overall reading ability and mathematics, by looking at the common characteristics responsible for high performance in mathematics and reading (e.g. De Maeyer et al.3 The current study goes further, because it investigates growth in reading comprehension and mathematics without any assumption on their functional dependence. The call for the use of multivariate methods to investigate the strength of a relationship between outcomes has been an invaluable problem in educational research as a whole.4,5

Reading achievement in primary school contains two distinguishable components: word reading and reading comprehension. Reading comprehension is a complex process and requires not only the fluent decoding of words but also understanding vocabulary, making inferences and relating the ideas to prior knowledge. Reading comprehension in primary schools stretches from the understanding of the meaning of words to the meaning of a short text and this is highly dependent on age. Many researchers have argued that true measurement of reading comprehension skills can only be obtained at later stages of primary school say from the third grade on (7 or 8 years old).6-8 This paper focuses on reading comprehension which requires more advanced cognitive and linguistic skills. For this reason pupils were only tested from the end of grade 3 on. While studies have shown that individual differences in reading comprehension in particular during primary school are stable,9 differences between schools10 however have not been addressed in depth. It might also be interesting to find out if this stability mentioned by (De Jong and Van Der Leij9) remains when considered in conjunction with mathematics.

Mathematics achievement in primary schools usually embodies different components like: arithmetic, basic number knowledge, memorisation of arithmetic operations like addition, subtraction, and conceptual understanding and procedural knowledge.11,12 Studies have shown that pupils’ skill in mathematics develops rapidly during formal instruction.13,14

Most studies in the field of educational effectiveness are limited to only one outcome variable as the effectiveness criterion. Or these studies report on several effectiveness criteria, but they model each criterion separately. Many researchers have called for the development of better statistical methods capable of handling more than one effectiveness criterion in the same model.10,15,16 When effectiveness criteria are modelled separately, the underlying assumption is that these different effectiveness criteria are independent of each other. As far as the school effects estimation is concerned, we hypothesize that this assumption is most often not tenable with reading comprehension and mathematics in primary school. The implication of such an assumption is for example, that a pupil’s or school’s score in mathematics is not related to the pupil’s or school’s performance in reading comprehension. On the contrary, some studies have suggested that mathematics achievement and reading as a whole may depend on similar predictors,17,18 strengthening the need for combining these effectiveness criteria. Other studies have even found that similar linguistic abilities are needed for both mathematics problem solving and reading comprehension tasks.19 Further studies have identified four components of mathematical problem solving as; translation, integration, solution planning and execution, with the first two of these components highly correlated with reading comprehension skills.20

In addition to the plea for multiple criteria, educational effectiveness researchers advocate studying student outcomes over time.2123 It is argued that growth in student outcomes over time is a very essential criterion since learning means changing.10 In order to investigate changes in student outcomes over time, longitudinal data are invaluable. This paper will address both the plea for multiple effectiveness criteria and the plea for studying growth. The use of growth curves as a statistical method introduces another level of choice of criterion beyond the choice of pupil outcome variable. This paper tries in the next section to delineate these two stages of school effectiveness criteria. Firstly, the choice of the dependent or outcome variables with a correlation strong enough to lend credence to a multivariate model instead of separate univariate models. Secondly, one has to make a choice of which growth curve parameters to use to model the student outcomes and to estimate the school effect. These parameters could be obtained at a certain point in time (intercept) or as a growth parameter (linear or quadratic). A bivariate transition multilevel growth curve model (BTMGCM) is introduced in this paper as a way of circumventing the problem of missing reading comprehension scores at the early grades.

Two-stage effectiveness criteria

Generalisation of results of different studies with different criteria has most often been problematic given that school effects are only moderately consistent over different criteria. Moreover, the consistency issue has seldom been studied through growth curve models. This paper introduces what is called “two-stage effectiveness criteria” to study school effect consistency in multivariate multilevel growth curve models (MMGCM). In stage 1, the researcher chooses one or more student outcomes (reading comprehension, mathematics achievement, well-being, etc.) and in stage 2, the growth parameters are chosen (initial status, linear change, quadratic slope, etc.). This process can result in two or more effectiveness criteria depending on the number of outcome variables and the nature of the growth. For example, two outcome variables and a random intercept and linear growth model will have four possible effectiveness criteria: an intercept (student status) and a linear slope (student growth) each for both outcomes. It is important to note that this two-stage effectiveness criteria is not limited to educational effectiveness research but can adequately be applied to other educational research domains as long as the researcher has a good longitudinal data on more than one outcome of interest.

The current study has two first stage criteria which are mathematics and reading comprehension and two second stage criteria (intercept and linear slope). The four effectiveness criteria in this study are therefore: mathematics intercept, mathematics slope, reading comprehension intercept and reading comprehension slope. These four effectiveness criteria (Figure 1- illustrate pupils' growth in both mathematics and reading comprehension (and their relation) in primary schools through a bivariate multilevel growth curve model (BMGCM).

Figure 1 A proposed schema of the two-stage school effectiveness criteria for a bivariate linear growth curve model.

The advantages of MMGCM are enormous. They are not only statistically powerful, but are also capable of answering a wider range of research questions more efficiently. These questions could stretch from stability to consistency of school effects. The choice of characteristics taking into account their dependence, can greatly reduce the chance of making a type 1 error (Hox, 2002) and as a consequence, improves generalisability of the study findings. The use of MMGCM is not limited to the investigation of cognitive outcomes but all non-cognitive outcomes and a mix of both. Some researchers have used a latent growth approach to looked into the relationship between two students’ outcomes, academic self-concept and language achievement.21,24

Objectives

The main objectives of the current study are summarised by the following research questions:

  1. What is the nature of the evolution of mathematics achievement and reading comprehension from the end of Grade 3 to the end of Grade 6 in primary school? Are there differences in the growth trajectories for these two outcomes?
  2. What is the correlation between the mathematics and reading comprehension growth profiles at the pupil level and at the school level?
  3. How large is the school effect on the pupil status and pupil growth? Is this effect similar for the two outcomes?
  4. What is the impact on the estimates of the school effects when the dependence of mathematics and reading comprehension is taken into account? In other words, we will compare school effects for two separate univariate models and one bivariate model.
  5. Can the prior growth in mathematics explain the subsequent differences in pupils’ status and growth in reading comprehension and mathematics achievement?

Methodology

In this section, we will discuss three models: (Model 1) the univariate multilevel growth curve model (UMGCM), (Model 2) the bivariate multilevel growth curve model (BMGCM) and (Model 3) the extension to a transition model (the bivariate transition multilevel growth curve model, BTMGCM). Model 1 will be used to answer the first research question, while model 2 will be used to tackle the second and third research questions. Research question four is based on the comparison of the results based on the Model 1 and 2. The last research question is then handled with model 3.

Univariate multilevel growth curve model (UMGCM)

The univariate multilevel growth curve models (UMGCM) will be applied to the mathematics scores on the one hand and the reading comprehension scores on the other hand. These two univariate multilevel growth curve models are each of the form

y i | b i ~N( X i β+ Z i b i , Σ i ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqzGeGaamyEaS WaaSbaaeaajugWaiaadMgaaSqabaqcfa4aaqqaaOqaaKqzGeGaamOy aSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaadMgaaSqabaaakiaawEa7aKqzGeGaaiOFai aad6eacaGGOaGaamiwaSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaadMgaaSqabaqcLbsa cqaHYoGycaaMc8Uaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadQfalmaaBaaabaqcLbmaca WGPbaaleqaaKqzGeGaamOyaSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaadMgaaSqabaqc LbsacaGGSaGaaGPaVlabfo6atTWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaadMgaaSqaba qcLbsacaGGPaaaaa@5B0A@ (1)

In equation 1 above, the y i MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqzGeGaamyEaS WaaSbaaeaajugWaiaadMgaaSqabaaaaa@39CB@ ’ s are vectors representing all the measurements for the i th MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaqcfaOaamyAam aaCaaajuaibeqaaabaaaaaaaaapeGaamiDaiaadIgaaaaaaa@39C8@  school.

Each outcome or response measurement Y= ( Y 1 ,  Y 2 ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbacbaaaaaaa aapeGaamywaiabg2da9iaabccapaWaaeWaaeaapeGaamywa8aadaWg aaqcfasaa8qacaaIXaaapaqabaqcfa4dbiaacYcacaqGGaGaamywa8 aadaWgaaqcfasaa8qacaaIYaaajuaGpaqabaaacaGLOaGaayzkaaaa aa@4174@  denotes the k th MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaqcfaieaaaaaa aaa8qacaWGRbWdamaaCaaabeqaa8qacaWG0bqcfaIaamiAaaaaaaa@39E9@  measurement for the j th MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaqcfaieaaaaaa aaa8qacaWGQbWdamaaCaaabeqcfasaa8qacaWG0bGaamiAaaaaaaa@39E8@  student from the i th MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVu0Je9sqqrpepC0xbbL8F4rqqrFfpeea0xe9Lq=Jc9 vqaqpepm0xbba9pwe9Q8fs0=yqaqpepae9pg0FirpepeKkFr0xfr=x fr=xb9adbaqaaeGaciGaaiaabeqaamaabaabaaGcbaqcfaOaamyAam aaCaaajuaibeqaaabaaaaaaaaapeGaamiDaiaadIgaaaaaaa@39C8@ school. This means the vector of responses

y i MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaadMhadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaaqabaaaaa@3803@ = ( y i11 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaadMhadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaiaaigdacaaIXaaabeaaaaa@3979@ , y i12 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaadMhadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaiaaigdacaaIYaaabeaaaaa@397A@ ,…, y i1 m j MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaadMhadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaiaaigdacaWGTbWaaSbaaWqaaiaadQgaaeqaaaWcbeaa aaa@3AD7@ ,…, y i n i m j MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaadMhadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaiaad6gadaWgaaadbaGaamyAaaqabaWccaWGTbWaaSba aWqaaiaadQgaaeqaaaWcbeaaaaa@3C35@ )T.

Bivariate multilevel growth curve model (BMGCM)

In the bivariate model, the two outcomes are combined through the proper specifications of a bivariate distribution for all the random effects taking into account the dependence of the growth processes. In this combined model, a bivariate normally distributed response is considered for the new response Y. Where Y= ( Y 1 ,  Y 2 ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbacbaaaaaaa aapeGaamywaiabg2da9iaabccapaWaaeWaaeaapeGaamywa8aadaWg aaqcfasaa8qacaaIXaaapaqabaqcfa4dbiaacYcacaqGGaGaamywa8 aadaWgaaqcfasaa8qacaaIYaaajuaGpaqabaaacaGLOaGaayzkaaaa aa@4174@ N(( X 1 β , 1 X 2 β 2 ),( Σ 1 , Σ 2 )) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakaad6eaca GGOaGaaiikaiaadIfadaWgaaqcfasaaiaaigdaaeqaaKqbakabek7a InaaBeaajuaibaGaaGymaaqabaqcfaOaaiilaiaadIfadaWgaaqcfa saaiaaikdaaKqbagqaaiabek7aInaaBaaajuaibaGaaGOmaaqcfaya baGaaiykaiaacYcacaaMc8Uaaiikaiabfo6atnaaBaaajuaibaGaaG ymaaqabaqcfaOaaiilaiabfo6atnaaBaaajuaibaGaaGOmaaqcfaya baGaaiykaiaacMcaaaa@508A@  and the mean structures, variance matrices that are allowed to be different and an extra covariance component as explained later on.

A multivariate response can be incorporated into a multilevel growth curve model by creating an extra lowest level, which is called level zero in this paper. In the growth curve model setting, the two responses are nested within the measurement occasions which are in turn nested within the students and finally within the schools. The main purpose of the level 0 is to define the double response per pupil. Our interest is then to use this model to assess the relationship between the growth parameters of the two response variables (reading comprehension and mathematics achievement) Figure 2.

Figure 2 Data collection structure for reading comprehension and mathematics outcome variables.

Modelling the two outcome variables simultaneously, accounts for the dependence between the outcomes and thus improves the parameter estimates of the model. This is usually of great importance when association structures change with time.25 In this study, we will fit a model, which has a structure of a four-level model but with the lowest level called level 0 because its variability is not of interest. The reason being that the level 0 index is used only to differentiate between the response variables. In this case the structure of the data fits into a multilevel growth curve model.

Y ijk = β 01 z 1ijk + β 02 z 2ijk + β 11 t ijk z 1ijk + β 12 t ijk z 2ijk + β 21 t ijk 2 z 1ijk + β 22 t ijk 2 z 2ijk + ( v 00k + v 10k t ijk + v 20k t ijk 2 + u 0ik + u 1ik t ijk + u 2ik t ijk 2 + ε ijk ) z 1ijk + (v ' 00k +v ' 10k t ijk +v ' 20k t ijk 2 +u ' 0ik +u ' 1ik t ijk +u ' 2ik t ijk 2 +ε ' ijk ) z 2ijk } MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbaoaaciaaea qabeaacaWGzbWaaSbaaKqbGeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaKqbagqa aiaaykW7cqGH9aqpcaaMc8UaeqOSdi2aaSbaaeaacaaIWaGaaGymaa qabaGaamOEamaaBaaajuaibaGaaGymaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaaqc fayabaGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7caaMc8UaeqOSdi2aaSbaaeaaca aIWaGaaGOmaaqabaGaamOEamaaBaaabaGaaGOmaiaadMgacaWGQbGa am4AaaqabaGaey4kaSIaaGPaVlabek7aInaaBaaabaGaaGymaiaaig daaeqaaiaadshadaqhaaqaamaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWG RbaajuaGbeaaaeaaaaGaamOEamaaBaaabaGaaGymaKqbGiaadMgaca WGQbGaam4AaaqcfayabaGaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaaykW7cqaHYoGydaWg aaqaaiaaigdacaaIYaaabeaajuaicaWG0bqcfa4aa0baaeaadaWgaa qcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaqcfayabaaabaaaaiaadQhadaWg aaqaaiaaikdacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaiaaykW7cqGHRaWkca aMc8UaeqOSdi2aaSbaaeaacaaIYaGaaGymaaqabaGaamiDamaaDaaa baWaaSbaaeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaqcfasaaiaaikdaaa qcfaOaamOEamaaBaaabaGaaGymaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaqabaGa aGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7caaMc8UaeqOSdi2aaSbaaeaacaaIYaGaaG OmaaqabaGaamiDamaaDaaabaWaaSbaaKqbGeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaa dUgaaKqbagqaaaqcfasaaiaaikdaaaqcfaOaamOEamaaBaaabaGaaG OmaKqbGiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaqcfayabaGaaGPaVlabgUcaRaqa aiaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8 UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7 caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVl aaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8Ua aGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7ca aMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caGGOaGaamODamaaBaaabaGaaGimaiaaicda caWGRbaabeaacaaMc8Uaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadAhadaWgaaqaaiaaig dacaaIWaGaam4AaaqabaGaamiDamaaDaaabaWaaSbaaKqbGeaacaWG PbGaamOAaiaadUgaaKqbagqaaaqaaaaacaaMc8Uaey4kaSIaaGPaVl aadAhadaWgaaqaaiaaikdacaaIWaGaam4AaaqabaGaamiDamaaDaaa baWaaSbaaKqbGeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaKqbagqaaaqaaiaaik daaaGaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadwhadaWgaaqaaiaaicdacaWGPbGaam4A aaqabaGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7caWG1bWaaSbaaeaacaaIXaGaam yAaiaadUgaaeqaaiaadshadaqhaaqaamaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaiaa dQgacaWGRbaajuaGbeaaaeaaaaGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7caWG1b WaaSbaaeaacaaIYaGaamyAaiaadUgaaeqaaiaadshadaqhaaqaamaa BaaajuaibaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaajuaGbeaaaeaacaaIYaaaai abgUcaRiabew7aLnaaBaaabaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaacaGG PaGaamOEamaaBaaabaGaaGymaKqbGiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaaqcfa yabaGaey4kaScabaGaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaM c8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaayk W7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPa VlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8 UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7 caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVl aaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caGGOaGaamODaiaacEcadaWgaaqa aiaaicdacaaIWaGaam4AaaqabaGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7caWG2b Gaai4jamaaBaaabaGaaGymaiaaicdacaWGRbaabeaajuaicaWG0bqc fa4aa0baaKqbGeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaa qabaaabaaaaKqbakaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaamODaiaacEcadaWg aaqaaiaaikdacaaIWaGaam4AaaqabaqcKvaG=laadshajuaGdaqhaa qcKvaG=haajuaGdaWgaaqcKvaG=haacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqa aaqaaiaaikdaaaqcfaOaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadwhacaGGNaWaaSbaae aacaaIWaGaamyAaiaadUgaaeqaaiaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaamyD aiaacEcadaWgaaqaaiaaigdacaWGPbGaam4AaaqabaqcfaIaamiDaK qbaoaaDaaajuaibaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUga aeqaaaqaaaaajuaGcaaMc8Uaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadwhacaGGNaWaaS baaeaacaaIYaGaamyAaiaadUgaaeqaaKqbGiaadshajuaGdaqhaaqc fasaaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaaaeaaca aIYaaaaKqbakabgUcaRiabew7aLjaacEcadaWgaaqaaiaadMgacaWG QbGaam4AaaqabaGaaiykaiaadQhadaWgaaqaaiaaikdacaWGPbqcfa IaamOAaiaadUgaaKqbagqaaiaaykW7aaGaayzFaaaaaa@D2AB@ (2)

where z 1ijk ={ 1ifReadingcomprehension 0ifMathematicsachievement z 2ijk ={ 0ifReadingcomprehension 1ifMathematicsachievement MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOabaeqabaGaamOEam aaBaaaleaacaaIXaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaakiabg2da9maa ceaabaqbaeqabiqaaaqaaiaaigdacaaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caWGPb GaamOzaiaaykW7caWGsbGaamyzaiaadggacaWGKbGaamyAaiaad6ga caWGNbGaaGPaVlaadogacaWGVbGaamyBaiaadchacaWGYbGaamyzai aadIgacaWGLbGaamOBaiaadohacaWGPbGaam4Baiaad6gacaaMc8Ua aGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVdqaaiaaic dacaaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaadMgacaWGMbGaaGPa Vlaad2eacaWGHbGaamiDaiaadIgacaWGLbGaamyBaiaadggacaWG0b GaamyAaiaadogacaWGZbGaaGPaVlaadggacaWGJbGaamiAaiaadMga caWGLbGaamODaiaadwgacaWGTbGaamyzaiaad6gacaWG0baaaaGaay 5EaaaabaGaamOEamaaBaaaleaacaaIYaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaa beaakiabg2da9maaceaabaqbaeqabiqaaaqaaiaaicdacaaMc8UaaG PaVlaaykW7caWGPbGaamOzaiaaykW7caWGsbGaamyzaiaadggacaWG KbGaamyAaiaad6gacaWGNbGaaGPaVlaadogacaWGVbGaamyBaiaadc hacaWGYbGaamyzaiaadIgacaWGLbGaamOBaiaadohacaWGPbGaam4B aiaad6gacaaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8 UaaGPaVdqaaiaaigdacaaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaa dMgacaWGMbGaaGPaVlaad2eacaWGHbGaamiDaiaadIgacaWGLbGaam yBaiaadggacaWG0bGaamyAaiaadogacaWGZbGaaGPaVlaadggacaWG JbGaamiAaiaadMgacaWGLbGaamODaiaadwgacaWGTbGaamyzaiaad6 gacaWG0baaaaGaay5Eaaaaaaa@D9CB@

This means our model can be written as

Y ijk ={ β 01 + β 11 t ijk + β 21 t ijk 2 + v 00k + v 10k t ijk + v 20k t ijk 2 + u 0ik + u 1ik t ijk + u 2ik t ijk 2 + ε ijk if z 1ijk =1 β 02 + β 12 t ijk + β 22 t ijk 2 +v ' 00k +v ' 10k t ijk +v ' 20k t ijk 2 +u ' 0ik +u ' 1ik t ijk +u ' 2ik t ijk 2 +ε ' ijk if z 2ijk =1 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakaadMfada WgaaqaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaqabaGaeyypa0Zaaiqaaeaafaqa beGabaaabaGaeqOSdi2aaSbaaeaacaaIWaGaaGymaaqabaGaaGPaVl abgUcaRiaaykW7cqaHYoGydaWgaaqaaiaaigdacaaIXaaabeaacaWG 0bWaa0baaKqbGeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaa qabaaabaaaaKqbakaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaeqOSdi2aaSbaaeaa caaIYaGaaGymaaqabaGaamiDamaaDaaajuaibaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGe aacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaqaaiaaikdaaaqcfaOaaGPaVlaa ykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaamODamaaBaaabaGaaGimaiaaicdacaWGRb aabeaacaaMc8Uaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadAhadaWgaaqaaiaaigdacaaI WaGaam4AaaqabaGaamiDamaaDaaajuaibaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaaca WGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaqcfayaaaaacaaMc8Uaey4kaSIaaGPa VlaadAhadaWgaaqaaiaaikdacaaIWaGaam4AaaqabaGaamiDamaaDa aajuaibaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaqa aiaaikdaaaqcfaOaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadwhadaWgaaqaaiaaicdaca WGPbGaam4AaaqabaGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7caaMc8UaamyDamaa BaaabaGaaGymaiaadMgacaWGRbaabeaajuaicaWG0bqcfa4aa0baaK qbGeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaaqabaaabaaa aKqbakaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaaGPaVlaadwhadaWgaaqaaiaaik dacaWGPbGaam4AaaqabaGaamiDamaaDaaajuaibaqcfa4aaSbaaKqb GeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaqaaiaaikdaaaqcfaOaaGPaVl abgUcaRiabew7aLnaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaa caaMc8EcfaOaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaG PaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaM c8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaamyAaiaadAgacaaMc8UaamOEamaaBa aabaGaaGymaiaadMgajuaicaWGQbGaam4AaaqcfayabaGaeyypa0Ja aGymaaqaaiabek7aInaaBaaabaGaaGimaiaaikdaaeqaaiabgUcaRi aaykW7cqaHYoGydaWgaaqaaiaaigdacaaIYaaabeaajuaicaWG0bqc fa4aa0baaKqbGeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaa qabaaabaaaaKqbakaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaeqOSdi2aaSbaaeaa caaIYaGaaGOmaaqabaGaamiDamaaDaaajuaibaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGe aacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaqaaiaaikdaaaqcfaOaey4kaSIa aGPaVlaadAhacaGGNaWaaSbaaeaacaaIWaGaaGimaiaadUgaaeqaai aaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaamODaiaacEcadaWgaaqaaiaaigdacaaI WaGaam4AaaqabaqcfaIaamiDaKqbaoaaDaaajuaibaqcfa4aaSbaaK qbGeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaqaaaaajuaGcaaMc8Uaey4k aSIaaGPaVlaadAhacaGGNaWaaSbaaeaacaaIYaGaaGimaiaadUgaae qaaKqbGiaadshajuaGdaqhaaqcfasaaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaamyA aiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaaaeaacaaIYaaaaKqbakabgUcaRiaaykW7ca WG1bGaai4jamaaBaaabaGaaGimaiaadMgacaWGRbaabeaacaaMc8Ua ey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadwhacaGGNaWaaSbaaeaacaaIXaGaamyAaiaadU gaaeqaaKqbGiaadshajuaGdaqhaaqcfasaaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGa amyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaaaeaaaaqcfaOaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaayk W7caWG1bGaai4jamaaBaaabaGaaGOmaiaadMgacaWGRbaabeaajuai caWG0bqcfa4aa0baaKqbGeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQb Gaam4AaaqabaaabaGaaGOmaaaajuaGcqGHRaWkcqaH1oqzjuaicaGG Naqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaKqbakaayk W7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caWGPbGaamOzaiaaykW7caWG6bWaaSba aeaacaaIYaGaamyAaKqbGiaadQgacaWGRbaajuaGbeaacqGH9aqpca aIXaGaaGPaVdaaaiaawUhaaaaa@5149@

The school level variance (level 3) is given by:

( v 00k v 10k v 20k v ' 00k v ' 10k v ' 20k )~MVN( ( 0 0 0 0 0 0 ),( σ v 0 2 σ v 1 v 0 σ v 1 2 σ v 2 v 0 σ v 2 v 1 σ v 2 2 σ v ' 0 v 0 σ v ' 0 v 1 σ v ' 0 v 2 σ v ' 0 2 σ v ' 1 v 0 σ v ' 1 v 1 σ v ' 1 v 2 σ v ' 1 v ' 0 σ v ' 1 2 σ v ' 2 v 0 σ v ' 2 v 1 σ v ' 2 v 2 σ v ' 2 v ' 0 σ v ' 2 v ' 1 σ v ' 2 2 ) ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbaoaabmaaba qbaeqabyqaaaaabaGaamODamaaBaaajuaibaGaaGimaiaaicdacaWG RbaajuaGbeaaaeaacaWG2bWaaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIXaGaaGimaiaadU gaaKqbagqaaaqaaiaadAhadaWgaaqcfasaaiaaikdacaaIWaGaam4A aaqcfayabaaabaGaamODaiaacEcadaWgaaqcfasaaiaaicdacaaIWa Gaam4AaaqcfayabaaabaGaamODaiaacEcadaWgaaqcfasaaiaaigda caaIWaGaam4AaaqcfayabaaabaGaamODaKqbGiaacEcajuaGdaWgaa qcfasaaiaaikdacaaIWaGaam4AaaqabaaaaaqcfaOaayjkaiaawMca aiaac6hacaWGnbGaamOvaiaad6eadaqadaqaamaabmaabaqbaeqaby qaaaaabaGaaGimaaqaaiaaicdaaeaacaaIWaaabaGaaGimaaqaaiaa icdaaeaacaaIWaaaaaGaayjkaiaawMcaaiaacYcadaqadaqaauaabe qagyaaaaaabaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaKqbGeaacaWG2bqcfa4aaSbaaKqb GeaacaaIWaaabeaaaeaacaaIYaaaaaqcfayaaaqaaaqaaaqaaaqaaa qaaiabeo8aZnaaDaaajuaibaGaamODaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGym aaqabaGaamODaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGimaaqabaaajuaGbaaaaa qaaiabeo8aZnaaDaaajuaibaGaamODaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGym aaqabaaabaGaaGOmaaaaaKqbagaaaeaaaeaaaeaaaeaacqaHdpWCda qhaaqcfasaaiaadAhajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaikdaaeqaaiaadAha juaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaicdaaeqaaaqaaaaaaKqbagaacqaHdpWCda qhaaqcfasaaiaadAhajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaikdaaeqaaiaadAha juaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaigdaaeqaaaqaaaaaaKqbagaacqaHdpWCda qhaaqcfasaaiaadAhajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaikdaaeqaaaqaaiaa ikdaaaaajuaGbaaabaaabaaabaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaKqbafaacaWG2b Gaai4jaKqbaoaaBaaajuaqbaGaaGimaaqabaGaamODaKqbaoaaBaaa juaqbaGaaGimaaqabaaajuaGbaaaaaqaaiabeo8aZnaaDaaajuaiba GaamODaiaacEcajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaicdaaeqaaiaadAhajuaG daWgaaqcfasaaiaaigdaaeqaaaqaaaaaaKqbagaacqaHdpWCdaqhaa qcfasaaiaadAhacaGGNaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIWaaabeaacaWG 2bqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIYaaabeaaaeaaaaaajuaGbaGaeq4Wdm 3aa0baaKqbGeaacaWG2bGaai4jaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGimaaqa baaabaGaaGOmaaaaaKqbagaaaeaaaeaacqaHdpWCdaqhaaqcfasaai aadAhacaGGNaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIXaaabeaacaWG2bqcfa4a aSbaaKqbGeaacaaIWaaabeaaaKqbagaaaaaabaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaK qbGeaacaWG2bGaai4jaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGymaaqabaGaamOD aKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGymaaqabaaajuaGbaaaaaqaaiabeo8aZn aaDaaajuaibaGaamODaiaacEcajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaigdaaeqa aiaadAhajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaikdaaeqaaaqcfayaaaaaaeaacq aHdpWCdaqhaaqcfasaaiaadAhacaGGNaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaI XaaabeaacaWG2bGaai4jaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGimaaqabaaaba aaaaqcfayaaiabeo8aZnaaDaaajuaibaGaamODaiaacEcajuaGdaWg aaqcfasaaiaaigdaaeqaaaqaaiaaikdaaaaajuaGbaaabaGaeq4Wdm 3aa0baaKqbGeaacaWG2bGaai4jaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGOmaaqa baGaamODaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaaGimaaqabaaajuaGbaaaaaqaai abeo8aZnaaDaaajuaibaGaamODaiaacEcajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaa ikdaaeqaaiaadAhajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaigdaaeqaaaqcfayaaa aaaeaacqaHdpWCdaqhaaqcfasaaiaadAhacaGGNaqcfa4aaSbaaKqb GeaacaaIYaaabeaacaWG2bqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIYaaabeaaaK qbagaaaaaabaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaKqbGeaacaWG2bGaai4jaKqbaoaa BaaajuaibaGaaGOmaaqabaGaamODaiaacEcajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaai aaicdaaeqaaaqaaaaaaKqbagaacqaHdpWCdaqhaaqcfasaaiaadAha caGGNaqcfa4aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIYaaabeaacaWG2bGaai4jaKqbao aaBaaajuaibaGaaGymaaqabaaajuaGbaaaaaqaaiabeo8aZnaaDaaa juaibaGaamODaiaacEcajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaaikdaaeqaaaqaai aaikdaaaaaaaqcfaOaayjkaiaawMcaaaGaayjkaiaawMcaaaaa@0667@ ,

and for the student level variance (level 2):

( u 0jk u 1jk u 2jk u ' 0jk u ' 1jk u ' 2jk )~MVN( ( 0 0 0 0 0 0 ),( σ u 0 2 σ u 1 u 0 σ u 1 2 σ u 2 u 0 σ u 2 u 1 σ u 2 2 σ u ' 0 u 0 σ u ' 0 u 1 σ u ' 0 u 2 σ u ' 0 2 σ u ' 1 u 0 σ u ' 1 u 1 σ u ' 1 u 2 σ u ' 1 u ' 0 σ u ' 1 2 σ u ' 2 u 0 σ u ' 2 u 1 σ u ' 2 u 2 σ u ' 2 u ' 0 σ u ' 2 u ' 1 σ u ' 2 2 ) ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaamaabmaabaqbae qabyqaaaaabaGaamyDamaaBaaaleaacaaIWaGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqa aaGcbaGaamyDamaaBaaaleaacaaIXaGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaGcba GaamyDamaaBaaaleaacaaIYaGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaGcbaGaamyD aiaacEcadaWgaaWcbaGaaGimaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaaaOqaaiaadw hacaGGNaWaaSbaaSqaaiaaigdacaWGQbGaam4AaaqabaaakeaacaWG 1bGaai4jamaaBaaaleaacaaIYaGaamOAaiaadUgaaeqaaaaaaOGaay jkaiaawMcaaiaac6hacaWGnbGaamOvaiaad6eadaqadaqaamaabmaa baqbaeqabyqaaaaabaGaaGimaaqaaiaaicdaaeaacaaIWaaabaGaaG imaaqaaiaaicdaaeaacaaIWaaaaaGaayjkaiaawMcaaiaacYcadaqa daqaauaabeqagyaaaaaabaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhadaWgaa adbaGaaGimaaqabaaaleaacaaIYaaaaOGaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8oa baaabaaabaaabaaabaaabaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhadaWgaa adbaGaaGymaaqabaWccaWG1bWaaSbaaWqaaiaaicdaaeqaaaWcbaaa aaGcbaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhadaWgaaadbaGaaGymaaqaba aaleaacaaIYaaaaOGaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8oabaaabaaabaaabaaa baGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhadaWgaaadbaGaaGOmaaqabaWcca WG1bWaaSbaaWqaaiaaicdaaeqaaaWcbaaaaaGcbaGaeq4Wdm3aa0ba aSqaaiaadwhadaWgaaadbaGaaGOmaaqabaWccaWG1bWaaSbaaWqaai aaigdaaeqaaaWcbaaaaaGcbaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhadaWg aaadbaGaaGOmaaqabaaaleaacaaIYaaaaOGaaGzaVlaaygW7caaMc8 UaaGPaVlaaykW7aeaaaeaaaeaaaeaacqaHdpWCdaqhaaWcbaGaamyD aiaacEcadaWgaaadbaGaaGimaaqabaWccaWG1bWaaSbaaWqaaiaaic daaeqaaaWcbaaaaaGcbaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhacaGGNaWa aSbaaWqaaiaaicdaaeqaaSGaamyDamaaBaaameaacaaIXaaabeaaaS qaaaaaaOqaaiabeo8aZnaaDaaaleaacaWG1bGaai4jamaaBaaameaa caaIWaaabeaaliaadwhadaWgaaadbaGaaGOmaaqabaaaleaaaaaake aacqaHdpWCdaqhaaWcbaGaamyDaiaacEcadaWgaaadbaGaaGimaaqa baaaleaacaaIYaaaaOGaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8oabaaabaaabaGaeq 4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhacaGGNaWaaSbaaWqaaiaaigdaaeqaaSGa amyDamaaBaaameaacaaIWaaabeaaaSqaaaaaaOqaaiabeo8aZnaaDa aaleaacaWG1bGaai4jamaaBaaameaacaaIXaaabeaaliaadwhadaWg aaadbaGaaGymaaqabaaaleaaaaaakeaacqaHdpWCdaqhaaWcbaGaam yDaiaacEcadaWgaaadbaGaaGymaaqabaWccaWG1bWaaSbaaWqaaiaa ikdaaeqaaaWcbaaaaaGcbaGaeq4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhacaGGNa WaaSbaaWqaaiaaigdaaeqaaSGaamyDaiaacEcadaWgaaadbaGaaGim aaqabaaaleaaaaaakeaacqaHdpWCdaqhaaWcbaGaamyDaiaacEcada WgaaadbaGaaGymaaqabaaaleaacaaIYaaaaOGaaGPaVlaaykW7caaM c8UaaGPaVdqaaaqaaiabeo8aZnaaDaaaleaacaWG1bGaai4jamaaBa aameaacaaIYaaabeaaliaadwhadaWgaaadbaGaaGimaaqabaaaleaa aaaakeaacqaHdpWCdaqhaaWcbaGaamyDaiaacEcadaWgaaadbaGaaG OmaaqabaWccaWG1bWaaSbaaWqaaiaaigdaaeqaaaWcbaaaaaGcbaGa eq4Wdm3aa0baaSqaaiaadwhacaGGNaWaaSbaaWqaaiaaikdaaeqaaS GaamyDamaaBaaameaacaaIYaaabeaaaSqaaaaaaOqaaiabeo8aZnaa DaaaleaacaWG1bGaai4jamaaBaaameaacaaIYaaabeaaliaadwhaca GGNaWaaSbaaWqaaiaaicdaaeqaaaWcbaaaaaGcbaGaeq4Wdm3aa0ba aSqaaiaadwhacaGGNaWaaSbaaWqaaiaaikdaaeqaaSGaamyDaiaacE cadaWgaaadbaGaaGymaaqabaaaleaaaaaakeaacqaHdpWCdaqhaaWc baGaamyDaiaacEcadaWgaaadbaGaaGOmaaqabaaaleaacaaIYaaaaO GaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8oaaaGaayjkaiaawMcaaaGaayjkaiaawMca aaaa@FD65@ .

The level 1 matrix components represent parameters associated with the error terms of the two growth processes

( ε ijk ε ijk' )~MVN( ( 0 0 ),( σ 1 2 σ 21 σ 2 2 ) ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbaoaabmaaba qbaeqabiqaaaqaaiabew7aLnaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWG RbaabeaaaKqbagaacqaH1oqzdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam 4AaiaacEcaaeqaaaaaaKqbakaawIcacaGLPaaacaGG+bGaamytaiaa dAfacaWGobWaaeWaaeaadaqadaqaauaabeqaceaaaeaacaaIWaaaba GaaGimaaaaaiaawIcacaGLPaaacaGGSaWaaeWaaeaafaqabeGacaaa baGaeq4Wdm3aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIXaaabeaajuaGdaahaaqcfasabe aacaaIYaaaaaqcfayaaaqaaiabeo8aZnaaBaaajuaibaGaaGOmaiaa igdaaKqbagqaaaqaaiabeo8aZnaaBaaajuaibaGaaGOmaaqabaqcfa 4aaWbaaKqbGeqabaGaaGOmaaaaaaaajuaGcaGLOaGaayzkaaaacaGL OaGaayzkaaaaaa@5BBC@

In vector notation we can simple write
ν k ~MVN(0, Ω v ), υ jk ~MVN(0, Ω u )and ε ijk ~MVN(0, Ω ε ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakabe27aUn aaBaaajuaibaGaam4AaaqabaqcfaOaaiOFaiaad2eacaWGwbGaamOt aiaacIcacaaIWaGaaiilaiabfM6axnaaBaaajuaibaGaamODaaqcfa yabaGaaiykaiaaykW7caGGSaGaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaeqyXdu3a aSbaaKqbGeaacaWGQbGaam4AaaqabaqcfaOaaiOFaiaad2eacaWGwb GaamOtaiaacIcacaaIWaGaaiilaiabfM6axnaaBaaajuaibaGaamyD aaqabaqcfaOaaiykaiaaykW7caaMc8Uaamyyaiaad6gacaWGKbGaaG PaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlabew7aLnaaBaaabaGaamyAaKqbGiaa dQgacaWGRbaajuaGbeaacaGG+bGaamytaiaadAfacaWGobGaaiikai aaicdacaGGSaGaeuyQdC1aaSbaaKqbGeaacqaH1oqzaeqaaKqbakaa cMcaaaa@745E@

Where 0 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakaaicdaaa a@3733@  is a zero mean vector and Ω v MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakabfM6axn aaBaaajuaibaGaamODaaqabaaaaa@3951@  and Ω u MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakabfM6axn aaBaaajuaibaGaamyDaaqcfayabaaaaa@39DE@  are respectively the covariance matrices for the school and student levels. An extension of this unconditional growth curve model to a conditional model is possible. A conditional growth curve model will include other covariates in addition to time.26 Conditional versions of the BMGCM can enable the estimation of general and specific effects for the combined responses or for each response in the model respectively. A quadratic growth model is presented above for completeness making it easier for the reader to follow a less complex application of its linear growth equivalent.

Bivariate transition multilevel growth curve model (BTMGCM)

A common problem with multivariate outcome data is the possibility of incomplete observations in the outcome vector. There are a number of reasons why some observations might be absent in a study. When incomplete observations are missing at random or even completely at random, maximum likelihood estimates obtained from multilevel growth curve models27 or the full maximum likelihood estimates for latent growth models,24 are still valid. However sometimes because of the design of the study, the statistical method used or the type of pupil outcomes to be considered, attritions occur in one outcome variable and not in the other. The situation in this study is summarized in Table 1 with the (X) indicating that a test was administered at that primary school grade. Students took a mathematics test at 7 occasions, while the reading comprehension test was administered at 4 occasions.

Outcome

Begin
grade1

End
grade1

End
grade2

End
grade3

End
grade4

End
grade5

End
grade6

Mathematics

X

X

X

X

X

X

X

Reading Comp

 

 

 

X

X

X

X

Table 1 Overview of the measurement occasions of the mathematics and reading comprehension tests

A bivariate transition multilevel growth curve model (BTMGCM) is introduced in this section as a way of circumventing the problem of missing reading comprehension scores at the beginning of grade 1, end of grades 1 and 2. This is considered as a better alternative to deleting the available mathematics scores obtained at those measurement occasions. The purpose of this model is to account for any possible dependence of the pupils reading comprehension and mathematics growth curves on these prior mathematics achievement scores.

Transition models are a specific class of conditional models. In a transition model, an outcome ( Y ijk MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaadMfadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaaaaa@39C2@ ) in a longitudinal sequence is described as a function of previous outcomes or history h ijk MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaahIgadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaaaaa@39D5@ = ( Y ij1 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaadMfadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaiaadQgacaaIXaaabeaaaaa@398D@ ,…, Y ijk1 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaiaadMfadaWgaa WcbaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbGaeyOeI0IaaGymaaqabaaaaa@3B6A@ ).28,29 The order of a transition model is the number of previous measurements that is still considered to influence the current outcome. This is a model which is simple to fit and understand yet strong enough to enable the investigation of the complex relationship that current processes have with their history. These models have been discussed in detail in textbooks such as Diggle et al.,28 Molenberghs and Verbeke30 and Fahrmeir and Tutz.29 However, extensions to handle more than one student outcome and in a multilevel growth curve model setting have never been done. It is in this context that the following BTMGCM is introduced, firstly, to solve the problem of unequal number of measurement occasions for the two pupil outcomes. And secondly, the model provides a powerful framework that can throw more light on the question of dependence of growth in one outcome on previous growth in a different outcome. The formulation of a bivariate transition model is given as follows:

Y ijk ={ β 01 + β 11 t ijk + β 21 t ijk 2 + κ 1 ( h ijk ,β)+ v 00k + v 10k t ijk + v 20k t ijk 2 + u 0ik + u 1ik t ijk + u 2ik t ijk 2 + ε ijk if z 1ijk =1 β 02 + β 12 t ijk + β 22 t ijk 2 + κ 2 ( h ijk ,β)+v ' 00k +v ' 10k t ijk +v ' 20k t ijk 2 +u ' 0ik +u ' 1ik t ijk +u ' 2ik t ijk 2 +ε ' ijk if z 2ijk =1 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqzGeGaamywaS WaaSbaaeaajugWaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaWcbeaajugibiabg2da 9KqbaoaaceaakeaajugibuaabeqaceaaaOqaaKqzGeGaeqOSdiwcfa 4aaSbaaSqaaKqzadGaaGimaiaaigdaaSqabaqcLbsacaaMc8Uaey4k aSIaaGPaVlabek7aITWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaaigdacaaIXaaaleqaaK qzadGaamiDaSWaa0baaeaadaWgaaadbaqcLbmacaWGPbGaamOAaiaa dUgaaWqabaaaleaaaaqcLbsacaaMc8Uaey4kaSIaaGPaVlabek7aIT WaaSbaaeaajugWaiaaikdacaaIXaaaleqaaKqzadGaamiDaSWaa0ba aeaadaWgaaadbaqcLbmacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaWqabaaaleaaju gWaiaaikdaaaqcLbsacaaMc8UaaGPaVlabgUcaRiabeQ7aRTWaaSba aeaajugWaiaaigdaaSqabaqcLbmacaGGOaGaamiAaSWaaSbaaeaaju gWaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaWcbeaajugWaiaacYcacqaHYoGycaGG PaqcLbsacqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaamODaSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaaicdaca aIWaGaam4AaaWcbeaajugibiaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaamODaSWa aSbaaeaajugWaiaaigdacaaIWaGaam4AaaWcbeaajugWaiaadshalm aaDaaabaWaaSbaaWqaaKqzadGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaameqaaaWc baaaaKqzadGaaGPaVNqzGeGaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadAhalmaaBaaaba qcLbmacaaIYaGaaGimaiaadUgaaSqabaqcLbmacaWG0bWcdaqhaaqa amaaBaaameaajugWaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaadbeaaaSqaaKqzad GaaGOmaaaajugibiabgUcaRiaaykW7caWG1bWcdaWgaaqaaKqzadGa aGimaiaadMgacaWGRbaaleqaaKqzGeGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7ca aMc8UaamyDaSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaaigdacaWGPbGaam4AaaWcbeaa jugWaiaadshalmaaDaaabaWaaSbaaWqaaKqzadGaamyAaiaadQgaca WGRbaameqaaaWcbaaaaKqzGeGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7caaMc8Ua amyDaSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaaikdacaWGPbGaam4AaaWcbeaajugWai aadshalmaaDaaabaWaaSbaaWqaaKqzadGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaa meqaaaWcbaqcLbmacaaIYaaaaKqzGeGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiabew7aLT WaaSbaaeaajugWaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaWcbeaajugibiaaykW7 caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVl aaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8Ua aGPaVlaaykW7caWGPbGaamOzaiaaykW7caWG6bWcdaWgaaqaaKqzad GaaGymaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaWcbeaajugibiabg2da9iaaigda aOqaaKqzGeGaeqOSdi2cdaWgaaqaaKqzadGaaGimaiaaikdaaSqaba qcLbsacqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaeqOSdi2cdaWgaaqaaKqzadGaaGymaiaa ikdaaSqabaqcLbmacaWG0bWcdaqhaaqaamaaBaaameaajugWaiaadM gacaWGQbGaam4AaaadbeaaaSqaaaaajugibiaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaM c8UaeqOSdi2cdaWgaaqaaKqzadGaaGOmaiaaikdaaSqabaqcLbmaca WG0bWcdaqhaaqaamaaBaaameaajugWaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaaad beaaaSqaaKqzadGaaGOmaaaajugibiabgUcaRiabeQ7aRTWaaSbaae aajugWaiaaikdaaSqabaqcLbmacaGGOaGaamiAaSWaaSbaaeaajugW aiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaWcbeaajugWaiaacYcacqaHYoGycaGGPa qcLbsacqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaamODaKqzadGaai4jaSWaaSbaaeaajugW aiaaicdacaaIWaGaam4AaaWcbeaajugibiaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8 UaamODaKqzadGaai4jaSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaaigdacaaIWaGaam4A aaWcbeaajugWaiaadshalmaaDaaabaWaaSbaaWqaaKqzadGaamyAai aadQgacaWGRbaameqaaaWcbaaaaKqzGeGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7 caWG2bqcLbmacaGGNaWcdaWgaaqaaKqzadGaaGOmaiaaicdacaWGRb aaleqaaKqzadGaamiDaSWaa0baaeaadaWgaaadbaqcLbmacaWGPbGa amOAaiaadUgaaWqabaaaleaajugWaiaaikdaaaqcLbsacqGHRaWkca aMc8UaamyDaKqzadGaai4jaSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaaicdacaWGPbGa am4AaaWcbeaajugibiaaykW7cqGHRaWkcaaMc8UaamyDaKqzadGaai 4jaSWaaSbaaeaajugWaiaaigdacaWGPbGaam4AaaWcbeaajugWaiaa dshalmaaDaaabaWaaSbaaWqaaKqzadGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaame qaaaWcbaaaaKqzadGaaGPaVNqzGeGaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadwhacaGG NaWcdaWgaaqaaKqzadGaaGOmaiaadMgacaWGRbaaleqaaKqzadGaam iDaSWaa0baaeaadaWgaaadbaqcLbmacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaWqa baaaleaajugWaiaaikdaaaqcLbsacqGHRaWkcqaH1oqzjugWaiaacE calmaaBaaabaqcLbmacaWGPbGaamOAaiaadUgaaSqabaqcLbmacaaM c8UaaGPaVNqzGeGaaGPaVlaaykW7caWGPbGaamOzaiaaykW7jugWai aadQhalmaaBaaabaqcLbmacaaIYaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaaleqa aKqzGeGaeyypa0JaaGymaiaaykW7aaaakiaawUhaaaaa@B6A5@

Where κ 1 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakabeQ7aRn aaBaaajuaibaGaaGymaaqcfayabaaaaa@39C3@ , κ 2 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakabeQ7aRn aaBaaajuaibaGaaGOmaaqcfayabaaaaa@39C4@  are functions (most often linear) of the history ( h ijk MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakaadIgada WgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaaqabaaaaa@3A82@ ). In the special case of this study where the history for both outcome variables is based on the first three time point measurements of mathematics, κ 1 = κ 2 = κ MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakabeQ7aRn aaBaaajuaibaGaaGymaaqabaqcfaOaeyypa0JaeqOUdS2aaSbaaKqb afaajuaGdaaadaqaaiaaikdaaiaawMYicaGLQmcaaeqaaiabg2da9i abeQ7aRnaaBaaabaaabeaaaaa@42DD@ . The β MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakabek7aIb aa@381A@ ’s indicate the possibility of separate models for the independent variables of the growth curve model. In compact form, the bivariate transitional growth curve model can be written as

y i | ( b i ,κ( h i ,β))~N( X i β+ Z i b i +κ( h i ,β), Σ i ) MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakaadMhada WgaaqcfasaaiaadMgaaeqaaKqbaoaaeeaabaGaaiikaiaadkgadaWg aaqcfasaaiaadMgaaeqaaaqcfaOaay5bSdGaaiilaiabeQ7aRjaacI cacaWGObWaaSbaaKqbGeaacaWGPbaabeaajuaGcaGGSaGaeqOSdiMa aiykaiaacMcacaGG+bGaamOtaiaacIcacaWGybWaaSbaaKqbGeaaca WGPbaabeaajuaGcqaHYoGycaaMc8Uaey4kaSIaaGPaVlaadQfadaWg aaqaamaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaaqabaaajuaGbeaacaWGIbWaaSbaaK qbGeaacaWGPbaabeaajuaGcqGHRaWkcqaH6oWAcaGGOaGaamiAamaa BaaajuaibaGaamyAaaqabaqcfaOaaiilaiabek7aIjaacMcacaGGSa GaaGPaVlabfo6atnaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaaqabaqcfaOaaiykaaaa @6711@ .

The next section proceeds with the application of the models described so far. First of all the paper examines if BMGCMs are more realistic and statistically backed to use instead of two separate UMGCMs. Next it compares the BMGCM with the bivariate growth model controlling for previous changes in mathematics using BTMGCM. The results from the three models are then investigated for any fundamental changes in the conclusions.

Application

The data used for this study were collected as part of a longitudinal research project to describe and explain pathways through primary education, SiBO (Schoolloopbanen Inhet Basis Onderwijs). There were about 200 Flemish schools that participated in this study, which started in September 2003 and followed one cohort of pupils throughout their career in primary school (Grade 1 to Grade 6). All the pupils took mathematics achievement tests at 7 occasions and reading comprehension tests (Dutch language) at four occasions (see previous section). Grade-appropriate tests with common scales for the reading comprehension and mathematics scores were obtained separately for four measurement occasions and seven measurement occasions respectively, using Item Response Theory. A number of background variables were also collected including: socio-economic status of the family, gender, language spoken at home, age and ethnic-cultural background. The sample used for this paper had 194 schools with 6250 pupils.

The average growth profile was explored for both mathematics and reading comprehension and looking at the deviance statistics and parsimony, we settled down to a linear growth model for both outcomes. The time variable (linear slope) is coded 0 for end of Grade 3, 1 for end of Grade 4, 2 for end of Grade 5 and 3 for end of Grade 6. This means the intercept (student status) is considered at the end of Grade 3. Two main software programmes are used in this study because of their different merits. First, SAS 9.1 (SAS Institute Inc., 2003) is used because of its PROC MIXED which is very flexible and suitable for fitting hierarchical linear models and growth curve models (Singer, 1998). Secondly, MLwiN 2.0231 provides a wide range of multilevel models together with plotting diagnostics. Alternative software like32 could equally be used to fit some of the models described in this paper.

Results

The results of the BMGCM reveal interesting improvements in the estimates of school effects and correlations in comparison with UMGCMs. First and foremost, the level-1 correlation for the joint growth processes for mathematics and reading comprehension is 0.17 and significant at a 5% level (p < 0.0001) indicating the need of fitting a bivariate model instead of two separate univariate models to the data.

Looking at the results of the first column of Table 2a for the student level of the UMGCM, it is clear that all the variance-covariance parameters are significant except for the covariance between the pupil’s status and growth in reading comprehension. This seems to indicate that the pupils’ level for reading comprehension at the end of grade 3 has no significant relationship with the pupils’ growth thereafter. However, the negative correlation between the pupils’ status in mathematics and growth in mathematics (-0.182) is significant at a 5% level. Pupils with a high mathematics score at the end of grade 3 generally grow less between the end of grade 3 and end of grade 6.

The second column of Table 2a shows the results of the BMGCM with four extra parameters rendering the possibility to answer many more research questions. The non-significant correlation between pupils’ status and growth in reading comprehension for the UMGCM is now significant under the BMGCM with value -0.085. This correlation is in the same direction as that between pupils’ mathematics status and growth though weaker. Also the effect of pupils’ status on growth in mathematics is significant and seems stronger in the BMGCM (-0.222) than in the UMGCM (-0.182). The cross co-variances were all significant with corresponding correlations of 0.691 between pupils’ mathematics intercept and reading comprehension intercept. Pupils with high scores in mathematics also tend to have high scores in reading comprehension at the end of grade 3.

Another positive correlation of 0.162 was observed between the pupils’ mathematics intercept and reading comprehension slope. This means the pupils with a high score in mathematics at the end of grade 3 tend to grow faster in reading comprehension subsequently. A correlation of 0.391 is estimated between pupils’ mathematics slope and reading comprehension slope. This indicates that fast growing pupils in mathematics also grew fast for reading comprehension. There is a significant and negative correlation for the pupils’ reading comprehension intercept and slope in mathematics (-0.095). The negative correlation means that high achievers in reading comprehension at the end of grade 3 generally had a slower growth in mathematics.

The results for the school level presented in Table 2b show some positive and significant correlations between the average mathematics and reading comprehension intercepts (0.672) and between their slopes (0.581). Schools with high end of year 3 scores in mathematics also have high scores in reading comprehension. Similarly schools with a steeper average slope in mathematics turn to have a steeper slope in reading comprehension too. The Table 2b results also show negative and significant correlations between the average intercept and average growth of schools for both mathematics and reading comprehension of -0.301 and -0.246 respectively. This means that schools with a high average mathematics score at the end of year 3 tend to have a slower average growth in mathematics during the subsequent grades, and so do the schools with high average reading comprehension. There were two correlations not significant at the 5% level for the relationship between schools’ average intercept in mathematics and average growth in reading comprehension and between the schools’ average intercept in reading comprehension and average growth in mathematics. These results may suggest that the school average growth in mathematics is not influenced by the average reading comprehension at the end of grade 3 and that the growth in reading comprehension is not influenced by the mathematics status too.

Variance parameter

UMGCM

BMGCM

Estimate

Std error

Correlation

Estimate

Std error

Correlation

Math status

57.783

1.254

1

67.629

1.435

1

Math slope

1.058

0.081

1

0.594

0.087

1

Read status

36.926

0.879

1

47.184

1.038

1

Read slope

0.861

0.081

1

0.888

0.083

1

Covariance parameter

 

 

 

 

 

 

Math status - slope

-1.425

0.251

-0.182

-1.408

0.293

-0.222

Read status - slope

-0.325

0.205

0.058

-0.555

0.243

-0.086

Math status - Read status

/

/

 

39.025

1.011

0.691

Math status - Read slope

/

/

 

1.255

0.279

0.162

Math slope - Read status

/

/

 

-0.501

0.263

-0.095

Math slope - Read slope

/

/

 

0.284

0.062

0.391

a) Student level variance parameters
Estimates in bold are not significant at a 5% level using a Wald test. Mat=mathematics, Read=reading comprehension, status is the student intercept at the end of grade 3 and slope= linear growth.

Variance parameter

UMGCM

BMGCM

Estimate

Std error

Correlation

Estimate

Std error

Correlation

Math status

17.853

2.121

1

15.44

1.917

1

Math slope

0.961

0.121

1

1.079

0.137

1

Read status

14.044

1.655

1

12.373

1.508

1

Read slope

0.456

0.068

1

0.676

0.093

1

Covariance parameter

Math status - slope

-1.665

0.39

-0.402

-1.231

0.383

-0.302

Read status - slope

-0.681

0.249

-0.269

-0.712

0.274

-0.246

Math status - Read status

/

/

9.277

1.439

0.671

Math status - Read slope

/

/

-0.05

0.299

-0.015

Math slope - Read status

/

/

-0.474

0.325

-0.13

Math slope - Read slope

/

/

0.497

0.09

0.582

b) School level variance parameters
Estimates in bold are not significant at a 5% level using a Wald test. Mat=mathematics, Read=reading comprehension, status is the student intercept at the end of grade 3 and slope= linear growth.

Table 2 Random Effects of the univariate multilevel growth curve models (UMGCM) compared with the bivariate multilevel growth curve model (BMGCM) with 4 measurements for Mathematics and Reading Comprehension

After considering that a bivariate growth model was the better model compared with two separate univariate growth models, the bivariate transition growth model was fitted to handle the difference in number of measurement occasions for reading comprehension and mathematics. The bivariate transition multilevel growth curve model (BTMGCM) is suggested in this study not only to solve inequality in the number of measurement occasions between the two outcome variables but also as a means of answering the fifth research question of the current study. In this special design of the transition model, the previous measurement covariates are constructed as changes in the mathematics achievement of the pupils between the beginning and end of first grade and between the end of grade 1 and the end of grade 2. Two such second order transition growth models are fitted. The BTMGCM (I) includes the two covariates (math2_1 and math3_2) as main effects only and the BTMGCM (II) adds the interactions between the two covariates and the time variable.

E( Y ijk )={ β 01 + β 11 t ijk + α 10 math2_1+ α 11 math32+ α 12 math2_1* t ijk + α 13 math3_2* t ijk if z 1ijk =1 β 02 + β 12 t ijk + α 20 math2_1+ α 21 math3_2+ α 22 math2_1* t ijk + α 23 math3_2* t ijk if z 2ijk =1 MathType@MTEF@5@5@+= feaagKart1ev2aaatCvAUfeBSjuyZL2yd9gzLbvyNv2CaerbuLwBLn hiov2DGi1BTfMBaeXatLxBI9gBaerbd9wDYLwzYbItLDharqqtubsr 4rNCHbGeaGqiVCI8FfYJH8YrFfeuY=Hhbbf9v8qqaqFr0xc9pk0xbb a9q8WqFfeaY=biLkVcLq=JHqpepeea0=as0Fb9pgeaYRXxe9vr0=vr 0=vqpWqaaeaabiGaciaacaqabeaadaqaaqaaaOqaaKqbakaadweaca GGOaGaamywamaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaajuaGbeaa caGGPaGaeyypa0ZaaiqaaeaafaqabeGabaaabaGaeqOSdi2aaSbaaK qbGeaacaaIWaGaaGymaaqcfayabaGaaGPaVlabgUcaRiaaykW7cqaH YoGydaWgaaqcfasaaiaaigdacaaIXaaabeaacaWG0bqcfa4aa0baaK qbGeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaaqabaaabaaa aKqbakaaykW7caaMc8Uaey4kaSIaeqySde2aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIXa GaaGimaaqabaGaamyBaiaadggacaWG0bGaamiAaiaaikdajuaGcaGG FbGaaGymaiabgUcaRiabeg7aHnaaBaaajuaibaGaaGymaiaaigdaae qaaiaad2gacaWGHbGaamiDaiaadIgacaaIZaGaeyOeI0IaaGOmaKqb akabgUcaRiaaykW7cqaHXoqydaWgaaqcfasaaiaaigdacaaIYaaabe aacaWGTbGaamyyaiaadshacaWGObGaaGOmaKqbakaac+facaaIXaGa aiOkaKqbGiaadshajuaGdaqhaaqcfasaaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaam yAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaaaeaaaaqcfaOaey4kaSIaeqySde2aaSba aKqbGeaacaaIXaGaaG4maaqabaGaamyBaiaadggacaWG0bGaamiAai aaiodacaGGFbGaaGOmaKqbakaacQcajuaicaWG0bqcfa4aa0baaKqb GeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaqabaaabaaaaK qbakaaykW7caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caaMc8UaamyAaiaadAgacaaM c8UaamOEamaaBaaabaGaaGymaKqbGiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4Aaaqcfa yabaGaeyypa0JaaGymaaqaaiabek7aInaaBaaajuaibaGaaGimaiaa ikdaaKqbagqaaiabgUcaRiaaykW7cqaHYoGydaWgaaqcfasaaiaaig dacaaIYaaabeaacaWG0bqcfa4aa0baaKqbGeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasa aiaadMgacaWGQbGaam4AaaqabaaabaaaaKqbakaaykW7cqGHRaWkcq aHXoqydaWgaaqcfasaaiaaikdacaaIWaaabeaacaWGTbGaamyyaiaa dshacaWGObGaaGOmaKqbakaac+facaaIXaGaey4kaSIaeqySde2aaS baaKqbGeaacaaIYaGaaGymaaqabaGaamyBaiaadggacaWG0bGaamiA aiaaiodajuaGcaGGFbGaaGOmaiabgUcaRiaaykW7cqaHXoqydaWgaa qcfasaaiaaikdacaaIYaaabeaacaWGTbGaamyyaiaadshacaWGObGa aGOmaKqbakaac+facaaIXaGaaiOkaKqbGiaadshajuaGdaqhaaqcfa saaKqbaoaaBaaajuaibaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaaaeaaaaqc faOaey4kaSIaeqySde2aaSbaaKqbGeaacaaIYaGaaG4maaqabaGaam yBaiaadggacaWG0bGaamiAaiaaiodacaGGFbGaaGOmaKqbakaacQca juaicaWG0bqcfa4aa0baaKqbGeaajuaGdaWgaaqcfasaaiaadMgaca WGQbGaam4AaaqabaaabaaaaiaaykW7juaGcaaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7 caaMc8UaaGPaVlaaykW7caWGPbGaamOzaiaaykW7caWG6bWaaSbaaK qbGeaacaaIYaGaamyAaiaadQgacaWGRbaabeaajuaGcqGH9aqpcaaI XaGaaGPaVdaaaiaawUhaaaaa@0487@

Estimates bold are not significant at a 5% level. Math2_1=change in mathematics between start of grade 1 and end of grade 1, Math3_2= change in mathematics between end of grade 1 and end of grade 2. Reading Comp=reading comprehension, status is the student intercept at the end of grade 3.

The results of Table 3 show quite some differences between the fixed effects estimates of the BMGCM and the two versions of the BTMGCM. The BTMGCM (I) considers the dependence on the main effects of the two previous growths in mathematics (Math2_1 and Math3_2) and BTMGCM (II) also includes the interaction effect of these previous changes with time. The deviance statistics can be used to compare the fits of the models using the difference in the log likelihood values and difference in degrees of freedom and a chi-square distribution as the null distribution for the likelihood ratio test statistic. Comparing BMGCM and BTMGCM (I) indicate a deviance value of 49506.3 with only 4 degrees of freedom is very significant (p < 0.0001) indicating that there will be a significant loss in information by trying to reduce the BTMGCM (I) to BMGCM. A similar conclusion is established between BTMGCM (I) and BTMGCM (II) in favour of BTMGCM (II) (p-value =0.017). The estimates of the earlier prior change in mathematics (start and end of grade 1) affect the pupils’ growth in both reading comprehension and mathematics from the end of grade 3 to the end of grade 6. On the other hand the later prior change in mathematics (between end of grade 1 and end of grade 2) seems to impact only the pupils’ subsequent growth in reading comprehension not their growth in mathematics.

Fixed effects parameter

BMGCM

BTMGCM (I)

BTMGCM (II)

Estimate (std error)

Estimate (std error)

Estimate (std error)

Intercept Reading Comp

43.224 (0.245)

46.051(0.362)

46.633 (0.395)

Intercept Mathematics

90.571 (0.312)

88.809 (0.479)

88.884 (0.497)

Time*Reading Comp

5.249 (0.068)

5.305 (0.071)

4.518 (0.135)

Time*Mathematics

5.563 (0.082)

5.584 (0.087)

5.248 (0.144)

Math2_1*Reading Comp

/

-0.145 (0.017)

-0.168 (0.017)

Math2_1*Mathematics

/

0.066 (0.019)

0.055 (0.018)

Math3_2*Reading Comp

/

-0.014 (0.019)

-0.039 (0.018)

Math3_2*Mathematics

/

0.179 (0.021)

0.185 (0.022)

Math2_1*Time*Read Comp

/

/

0.030 (0.005)

Math2_1*Time*Mathematics

/

/

0.018 (0.005)

Math3_2*Time* Read Comp

/

/

0.033 (0.005)

Math3_2*Time*Mathematics

/

/

0.008 (0.006)

-2loglikelihood

238524.3

189018.9

189005.5

Degrees of freedom

28

32

36

Table 3 Comparing the fixed effects estimates and model fits of a bivariate multilevel growth curve model (BMGCM) and two bivariate transitional multilevel growth curve models (BTMGCM (I) and BTMGCM (II))

Estimates in bold are not significant at a 5% level. Math2_1=change in mathematics between start of grade 1 and end of grade 1, Math3_2= change in mathematics between end of grade 1 and end of grade 2. Reading Comp=reading comprehension, status is the student intercept at the end of grade 3.

A summary of the fixed effects result is presented as growth profiles for the UMGCM, BMGCM and the final bivariate transition growth curve model in Figure 3. The graphs of Figure 3(a) reveal that the average pupil score in reading comprehension at the end of grade 3 is underestimated by the univariate model (Read UMGCM) and improved by the bivariate model (Read BMGCM). In any case, the BTMGCM (for pupils with 1SD difference in prior math achievement (ReadBTMGCM+1SD) for both Math2_1 and Math3_2), gives the largest estimates for the reading comprehension score at the end of grade 3. A possible explanation for the underestimation by the UMGCM and BMGCM is that these models assume falsely that the growth processes only started at the end of grade 3. The BTMGCM (II) seems to indicate that growth in mathematics of the previous grades has a role to play in the average reading comprehension performance of pupils at the end of the third grade.

The graphs of Figure 3(b) show no big difference between the BMGCM and BTMGCM for their estimates of the average score in mathematics at the end of grade 3. This means that the change in prior mathematics achievement has a bigger influence on pupils’ subsequent development in reading comprehension than in mathematics.

Figure 3 Average growth profiles for the UMGCM, BMGCM and BTMGCM (for +1SD difference in prior change) a) reading comprehension and b) mathematics.

Also the growth in pupils’ reading comprehension is larger in the BMGCM probably because this model assumes that this growth is only due to the instruction for reading comprehension. There seems to be quite a bit of contribution of skills gained due to the first two years of mathematics instruction to subsequent performances in reading comprehension.

Figure 4(a) shows that on the one hand , pupils with a plus one standard deviation difference (+1SD) in prior mathematics (Math2_1) achievement (ReadBTMGCM+1SD) tend to have a lower end of grade 3 reading comprehension score but a steeper growth subsequently. On the other hand it indicates that, pupils with minus one standard deviation difference (ReadBTMGCM-1SD), show a less steep growth in reading comprehension. Not surprising though, pupils with a +1SD difference (MathBTMGCM+1SD) in prior mathematics achievement showed a steeper growth than pupils with a -1SD difference (MathBTMGCM-1SD) from the end of grade3 to the end of grade 6.

Figure 4 BTMGCM profiles for pupils with +1SD and -1SD of difference in prior mathematics achievement a) Reading comprehension and b) Mathematics.

After considering the BTMGCM with earlier and later prior change in mathematics achievement as covariates, the results of variance-covariance parameters of the BMGCM are then compared with those of the BTMGCM and presented in Table 4.

Variance parameter

BMGCM

BTMGCM (I)

Estimate

Std error

Correlation

Estimate

Std error

Correlation

Math status

67.629

1.435

1

62.776

1.52

1

Math slope

0.594

0.087

1

0.599

0.094

1

Read status

47.184

1.038

1

46.355

1.165

1

Read slope

0.888

0.083

1

0.882

0.091

1

Covariance parameter

Math status – Math slope

-1.408

0.293

-0.222

-1.884

0.318

-0.307

Read status – Read slope

-0.555

0.243

-0.086

-0.552

0.268

-0.086

Math status - Read status

39.025

1.011

0.691

37.922

1.107

0.703

Math status - Read slope

1.255

0.279

0.162

0.984

0.307

0.132

Math slope - Read status

-0.501

0.263

-0.095

-0.667

0.254

-0.127

Math slope - Read slope

0.284

0.062

0.391

0.245

0.067

0.337

a) Student level variance parameters
Math=mathematics, Read=reading comprehension, status is the student intercept at the end of grade 3 and slope= linear growth.

Variance parameter

BMGCM

BTMGCM (I)

Estimate

Std error

Correlation

Estimate

Std error

Correlation

Math status

15.44

1.917

1

13.746

1.839

1

Math slope

1.079

0.137

1

1.177

0.158

1

Read status

12.373

1.508

1

9.391

1.3

1

Read slope

0.676

0.093

1

0.676

0.099

1

Covariance parameter

Math status – Math slope

-1.231

0.383

-0.302

-1.606

0.408

-0.399

Read status – Read slope

-0.712

0.274

-0.246

-0.766

0.266

-0.304

Math status - Read status

9.277

1.439

0.671

8.023

1.325

0.706

Math status - Read slope

-0.05

0.299

-0.015

-0.303

0.301

-0.099

Math slope - Read status

-0.474

0.325

-0.13

-0.893

0.329

-0.268

Math slope - Read slope

0.497

0.09

0.582

0.579

0.102

0.649

b) School level variance parameters
Estimates in bold are not significant at a 5% level. Math=mathematics, Read=reading comprehension, status is the student intercept at the end of grade 3 and slope= linear growth.

Table 4 Estimates of variance and covariance components for the bivariate multilevel growth curve model (BMGCM) compared with the bivariate transition multilevel growth curve model (BTMGCM (I))

The results are quite similar in terms of the direction of the covariance though the BMGCM seem to yield higher estimates for most of the parameters as compared to the BTMGCM (I). However, one conspicuous difference is the significant correlation (-0.268) between the average school intercept in reading comprehension and average slope in mathematics for the BTMGCM, which is not significant in the BMGCM. This means schools with a higher average score in reading comprehension at the end of grade 3 do not grow as fast in mathematics in comparison to schools with a lower average score. In other words, the higher the school’s average reading comprehension score at the end of grade 3, the lower the growth of the school in mathematics from the end of grade 3 to the end of grade 6.

The school effect estimates for mathematics and reading comprehension on the pupils’ status and growth were obtained for the different growth models described previously in this paper. The school effects are estimated as the proportion of variance accounted by the school level compared to that at the pupil level.25 The results of the school effect estimates under the three different models are summarised in Table 5 below. The results indicate that univariate growth curve models seem to overestimate the effect of schools on the pupils at the end of grade 3 for both reading comprehension (27.5%) and mathematics (23.6%). The improved BMGCM estimates the same effects as 22.8% for reading comprehension and 18.6% for mathematics. However, the UMGCM seem to underestimate the school effect on growth with estimates of 34.6% and 47.6% for reading comprehension and mathematics respectively. Looking at the same estimates using the BMGCM, they increase to 43.2% and 64.5% for reading comprehension and mathematics respectively. This means that assuming a joint bivariate growth in the pupils reading comprehension and mathematics achievement can result in larger estimates of the effects of schools on the pupils’ growth than treating the outcomes as if they were independent of each other.

At the end of Grade 3

Reading comprehension

School effect on student status

27.5%

22.8%

16.8%

School effect on student linear growth

34.6%

43.2%

43.4%

Mathematics achievement

School effect on student status

23.6%

18.6%

18.0%

School effect on student linear growth

47.6%

64.5%

66.5%

Table 5 The school effect estimates for the pupils’ status and growth in reading comprehension and mathematics at the end of grade 3, for the three growth curve models described

The BTMGCM is fitted to remove the false assumption that the bivariate growth process started at the end of grade 3 for both pupil outcomes ignoring the first three measurements of mathematics. It is possible that knowledge acquired during the first two grades of primary school, might be responsible not only in the better fit of the model as shown earlier but also help avoid the fallacy of missing at random assumption for this data structure. With the BTMGCM, changes were observed in the school effect estimates on the pupils’ status in reading comprehension (16.8%) and the growth in mathematics (66.5%).

This Table 5 also indicates that schools have a larger effect on their pupils’ mathematics growth than on their growth in reading comprehension.

Discussion

Considering the correlation results, the UMGCM shows no significant relationship between the pupils’ status and growth in reading comprehension. The UMGCM also underestimates the effect of schools on pupils’ linear change in mathematics as well as reading comprehension. However, this effect became significant in the BMGCM indicating better power in the latter model. The BMGCM results also illustrate that pupils’ who are higher achievers in mathematics at the end of grade 3 are also higher achievers in reading comprehension at the end of grade 3. Pupils with a stronger growth in mathematics also show a stronger growth in reading comprehension. Pupils who score high in mathematics at the end of grade 3 grow more in reading comprehension subsequently. However, pupils who are higher achievers in reading comprehension at the end of grade 3 experience a slower growth in mathematics.

At the school level, the correlation between the average reading comprehension at the end of grade 3 and the average mathematics at the end of grade 3 was significant. This implies schools with high achieving pupils in mathematics also have high achievers in reading comprehension. Also schools with a high average growth in reading comprehension have a high average growth in mathematics.

The significant correlation between the overall mathematics achievement and reading comprehension growth profiles is an indication that statistically a BMGCM approach is more appropriate. The BMGCM also resulted in about 35% increase in the school effect estimate on pupils’ growth in mathematics and about 25% increase for pupils’ growth in reading comprehension. However, the school effect on the pupils’ status dropped by 21% and 17% for mathematics and reading comprehension respectively. The univariate model seems to overestimate the differences between schools by the end of grade 3. The UMGCM also underestimates the effect of schools on pupils’ linear change in mathematics as well as reading comprehension. This is far enough evidence of the need for more than one criterion to better estimate the effects of schools on primary school children. The results of the BMGCM also show more clearly that the effect of schools is more pronounced on the pupils’ growth criterion than on the status. The school effects on the pupils’ growth is about 3.7 times larger than on the pupils’ status at the end of grade 3 for mathematics achievement and about 2.6 times more for reading comprehension. The same comparison at the beginning of grade 3 indicates that the school effect on pupils’ growth is about 1.9 times for mathematics and about 1.5 times for reading comprehension. Analysis at the end of grade 6 (not presented) corroborates these findings and also results of previous studies on longitudinal data.22,33 It is nonetheless advisable to fit models with many more first stage criteria than just two as in this study (mathematics and reading comprehension), in order to generalise these findings. This can be considered a relative advantage of the choice of the growth criterion over the status criterion at the second stage for both first stage criteria because schools seem to have more impact on it.

The results of the transition model showed that changes in mathematics achievement in the first and second grade could predict the change in reading comprehension in the later grades. When prior growth in mathematics is taken into account, the schools seem to help the low achievers at the end of grade 3 to catch up with their higher achieving peers by the end of grade 6. In other words pupils with a larger gain in earlier mathematics achievement grow faster in reading comprehension subsequently though they are low achievers at the end of grade 3.

The BMGCM showed no significant correlation between the schools’ average mathematics score at the end of grade 3 and the growth in reading comprehension. A similar non-significant result is obtained for the correlation between the school’s average score in reading comprehension at the end of grade 3 and the growth in mathematics. However, with the introduction of the BTMGCM, the correlation between the schools’ average score in reading comprehension and the schools average growth in mathematics became significant. The school level correlations indicate that schools that are effective in the pupils’ average mathematics achievement are also effective in their average reading comprehension attainment on both the status and growth criteria.

From the findings of this study, we recommend that researchers in the field of school effectiveness should consider multiple criteria to enable this field of research come up with improved school effect estimates. We encourage researchers to make more use of longitudinal data and the two-stage criteria proposed, to enable researchers to answer a wider range of scientifically relevant questions to school effectiveness research. We acknowledge the computational difficulties that such complex models with multiple stage 1 and 2 criteria will bring while recognising the invaluable contribution it will make to the field of educational effectiveness. The multivariate transition model proposed in this paper can be used by researchers to avoid false missing assumptions or even the loss of data and in addition can answer other very relevant research questions.

We were not able in our study to investigate the more appropriate joint causal change relationship. It would have been interesting for example to investigate whether the change in mathematics influences the change in reading comprehension and not the other way around. Nevertheless, this study serves as a strong foundation on which more complex educational research methods can be developed.3441

Acknowledgments

None.

Conflicts of interest

Author declares that there are no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Lerkkanen MK, Rasku-Puttonen H, Aunola K, et al. Mathematical performance predicts progress in reading comprehension among 7-years olds. European Journal of Psychology of Education. 2005;20(2):121–137.
  2. Kirsch I, De Jong J, LaFontaine D, et al. Reading for change. Performance and engagement across countries: Results from PISA 2000. 2002.
  3. De Maeyer S, Van den Bergh H, Rymenans R, et al. Effectiveness criteria in school effectiveness studies: Further research on the choice for a multivariate model. Educational Research Review. 2010;5(1):81–96.
  4. Phan HP. Deep processing strategies and critical thinking: Developmental trajectories using latent growth analyses. The Journal of Educational Research. 2011;104(4):283–294.
  5. Phan HP. Examination of self-efficacy and hope: A developmental approach using latent growth modeling. The Journal of Educational Research. 2013;106(2):93–104.
  6. Adams MJ, Treiman R, Pressley M. Reading, writing and literacy. 1998.
  7. Topping KJ, Fisher AM. Computerised formative assessment of reading comprehension: field trials in the UK. Journal of Research in Reading. 2003;26(3):267–279.
  8. Van Keer H. Fostering reading comprehension in fifth grade by explicit instruction in reading strategies and peer tutoring. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2004;74(1):37–70.
  9. De Jong PF, Van Der Leij A. Effects of phonological abilities and linguistic comprehension on the development of reading. Scientific Studies of Reading. 2002;6(1):51–77.
  10. Teddlie C, Reynolds D, Sammons P. The methodology and scientific properties of school effectiveness research. 2000.
  11. Dowker A. Individual differences in normal arithmetical development. 1998.
  12. Donlan C, Gourlay S. The importance of non-verbal skills in the acquisition of place-value knowledge: Evidence from normally-developing and language-impaired children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 1999;17(1):1–19.
  13. Geary DC. A componential analysis of an early learning deficit in mathematics. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1990;49: 363–-383.
  14. Jordan NC, Hanich LB, Kaplan D. A longitudinal study of mathematical competencies in children with specific mathematics difficulties versus children with comorbid mathematics and reading difficulties. Child Dev. 2003;74(3):834–850.
  15. Richardson J. Measuring the relationship between scores on two questionnaires. Educational Research Review. 2007;2(1):13–27.
  16. Van degaer E, De Fraine B, Pustjens H, et al. School effects on the development of motivation toward learning tasks and the development of academic self-concept in secondary education: a multivariate latent growth curve approach. School Effectiveness and School Improvement. 2009;20(2):235–253.
  17. Bull R, Johnston RS. Children’s arithmetical difficulties: Contributions from processing speed, item identification, and short-term memory. J Exp Child Psychol. 1997;65:1–24.
  18. Tymms P. Baseline assessment, value-added and the prediction of reading.Journal of Research in Reading. 1999;22(1):27–36.
  19. Jordan NC, Kaplan D, Hanich LB. Achievement growth in children with learning difficulties in mathematics: Findings of a two-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2002;94(3):586–597.
  20. Mayer RE. Educational psychology: A cognitive approach. Boston: Little, Brown; 1987.
  21. De Fraine B, Van Damme J, Onghena P. A longitudinal analysis of gender differences in academic self-concept and language achievement. A multivariate latent growth curve approach. Contemporary Educational Psychology. 2007;32(1):132–150.
  22. Raudenbush SW. The analysis of longitudinal, multilevel data. International Journal of Educational Research. 1989;13(7):721–740.
  23. Singer JD, Willett JB. Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modelling change and event occurrence. New York: Oxford University Press; 2003.
  24. Fieuws S, Verbeke G. Joint modeling of multivariate longitudinal profiles: pitfalls of the random effects approach. Statistics in Medicine. 2004;23(20):3093–3104.
  25. Anumendem D, De Fraine B, Onghena P, et al. The impact of coding time on the estimation of school effects. Quality and Quantity. 2013;47(2):1021–1040.
  26. Dempster AP, Laird NM, Rubin DB. Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via the EM algorithm (with discussion). Journal of the Royal Statistics Society. 1977;39:1–38.
  27. Muthen B. Latent variable analysis. Growth mixture modelling and related techniques for longitudinal data. 2004.
  28. Diggle PJ, Heagerty PJ, Liang KY, et al. Analysis of longitudinal data (2nd edn). Oxford Science Publications, Oxford: Clarendon Press; 2002.
  29. Fahrmeir L, Tutz G. Multivariate statistical modelling based on generalized linear models. 2nd ed. Springer Series in Statistics, New-York: Springer-Verlag; 2001.
  30. Molenberghs G, Verbeke G. Models for discrete longitudinal data. Springer Series in Statistics. New-York: Springer-Verlag. 2006.
  31. Rasbash J, Browne W, Goldstein H, et al. A user’s guide to MlwiN. London: Institute of Education. 2000.
  32. Muthen LK, Muthen BO. Growth modeling with latent variables using Mplus. 2000.
  33. Raudenbush SW. Statistical models for studying the effects of social context on individual development. 1995.
  34. Sigel IE, Renninger KA. Handbook of child psychology 4: Child psychology in practice. New York: Wiley. 2006;275–355.
  35. In C. The development of mathematical skills. Donlan. ed, East Sussex, UK. Psychology Press; 275–302.
  36. Hox J. Multilevel analysis. Techniques and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 2002.
  37. Kaplan D. The Sage handbook of quantitative methodology in the social sciences. London: Sage. pp. 345–368.
  38. Gottman JM, Rushe HR. The analysis of change: Issues, Fallacies, and New Ideas. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology.1993;61(6):907–910.
  39. SAS Institute Inc. SAS User’s Guide: Statistics. 9th ed. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc. 2007.
  40. Singer JD. Using SAS proc mixed to fit multilevel models, hierarchical models, and individual growth models. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics. 1998;23(4):323–355.
  41. Teddlie C, Reynolds D. The international handbook of school effectiveness research. London: Falmer Press. 1999;55–133.
Creative Commons Attribution License

©2017 Anumendem, et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and build upon your work non-commercially.